Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 36, Issue 4, pp 348–357 | Cite as

Emotional Interference in Working Memory is Related to Rumination

  • Evi De Lissnyder
  • Ernst H. W. Koster
  • Rudi De Raedt
Original Article

Abstract

Impairments in the executive control function of working memory may underlie moodcongruent attentional and memory biases in depression. Given that the capacity to control and manipulate information held in working memory has been implicated in depressive symptoms and rumination, the present study investigated shifting capacity between internal emotional and nonemotional information in working memory. For this purpose, the Internal Shift Task (IST) was administered to a sample of dysphoric (N = 20) and non-dysphoric undergraduates (N = 20). The main finding was that depressive symptoms in general were not related to impairments in shifting. Interestingly, rumination was related to internal shifting impairments that were most pronounced when negative information was held in working memory. These findings concerning the relation between rumination and shifting impairments are discussed in relation to cognitive vulnerability for depression.

Keywords

Depression Rumination Working memory Cognitive control Switching 

References

  1. Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan, M. E., Witehouse, W. G., Rose, D. T., Robinson, M. S., et al. (2000). The temple-Wisconsin cognitive vulnerability to depression project: Lifetime history of Axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 403–418.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amado-Boccara, I., Gougoulis, N., Poirier, L. M., Galinowski, A., & Loo, H. (1995). Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functions: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 479–493.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baert, S., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2011). Modification of information-processing biases in emotional disorders: Clinically relevant developments in experimental psychopathology. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy (in press). Google Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  6. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (2 nd ed.). The Psychological Association: San Antionio, TX.Google Scholar
  7. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychological Review, 8, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bernblum, R., & Mor, N. (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion, 10(3), 423–432.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy for depression. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
  11. Daches, S., Mor, N., Winquist, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2010). Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: Evidence from a modified Garner Task. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 876–885. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 699–711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 264–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Understanding increasing vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual model. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 50–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Deveney, C. M., & Deldin, P. J. (2006). A preliminary investigation of cognitive flexibility for emotional information in major depressive disorders and non-psychiatric controls. Emotion, 6, 429–437.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Ridder, E. M., & Beautrais, A. L. (2005). Subthreshold depression in adolescence and mental health outcomes in adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 66–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Ben-Artzi, E., Jeczemien, P., Marom, S., & Hermesh, H. (2004). Depression impairs the ability to ignore the emotional aspects of facial expressions: Evidence from the Garner task. Cognition & Emotion, 18(2), 209–231.Google Scholar
  19. Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. H. W. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, 149–157.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: A validation study. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 1094–1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. I. (2002). Handbook of Depression. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
  22. Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B., et al. (2004). Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depressions. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38, 567–576.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hertel, P. T. (2007). Impairments in inhibition or cognitive control in psychological disorders. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 149–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jonides, J., Lewis, R. L., Nee, D. E., Lusting, C. A., Berman, M. G., & Moore, K. S. (2007). The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 125–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Joormann, J. (2006). The relation of rumination and inhibition: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 149–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269–280.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 80–85.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12, 128–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangaz, K. R., et al. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 3095–3105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review.Google Scholar
  32. Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and recall bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 219–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF). Karolinska Hospital, Stockholm: Department of Neurosciences.Google Scholar
  34. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Merriam, E. P., Thase, M. E., Haas, G. L., Keshavan, M. S., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in depression determined by Wisconsin card sorting performance. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 780–782.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Monsell, S. (1996). Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce (Ed.), Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition (pp. 93–148). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consiousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Patel, T., Brewin, C. R., Wheatley, J., Wells, A., Fisher, P., & Myers, S. (2007). Intrusive images and memories in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2573–2580.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Raes, F., & Hermans, D. (2007). The revised version of the Dutch Ruminative Response Scale. Unpublished instrument.Google Scholar
  43. Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: een herziening van de Ruminative Response Scale. [‘Reflection’ and ‘brooding’ as two subtypes of rumination: A revision of the Ruminative Response Scale]. Gedragstherapie, 42, 205–214.Google Scholar
  44. Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 262–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Solomon, D., Keller, M., Leon, A., Mueller, T., Lavori, P., Shea, M., et al. (2000). Multiple recurrences of major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 229–233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck Depression Inventory, 2de Editie (The Dutch version of the Beck Depression Inventory, 2 nd Edition). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger b.v.Google Scholar
  48. Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72, 400–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18, 546–553.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Evi De Lissnyder
    • 1
  • Ernst H. W. Koster
    • 1
  • Rudi De Raedt
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Experimental Clinical and Health PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium

Personalised recommendations