Skip to main content
Log in

The Character Lens: A Person-Centered Perspective on Moral Recognition and Ethical Decision-Making

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We introduce the character lens perspective to account for stable patterns in the way that individuals make sense of and construct the ethical choices and situations they face. We propose that the way that individuals make sense of their present experience is an enduring feature of their broader moral character, and that differences between people in ethical decision-making are traceable to upstream differences in the way that people disambiguate and give meaning to their present context. In three studies, we found that individuals with higher standing on moral character (operationalized as a combination of Honesty-Humility, Guilt Proneness, and Moral Identity Centrality) tended to construe their present context in more moral or ethical terms, and this difference in moral recognition accounted for differences in the ethical choices they made. Moreover, individuals with higher levels of moral character maintained high levels of moral recognition even as pressure to ignore moral considerations increased. Accordingly, this work unifies research on moral character, moral recognition, sensemaking, and judgment and decision-making into a person-centered account of ethical decision-making, highlighting the way decision-makers actively and directly shape the choice contexts to which they must respond.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the literature, moral awareness (Jordan, 2009; Reynolds, 2006) refers to an individual’s recognition that a single situation contains moral issues, whereas moral sensitivity (Sparks, 2015; Sparks & Hunt, 1998) refers to a broader recognition of moral issues across multiple situations (Reynolds & Miller, 2015). The terms moral construal (van Bavel et al., 2012) and ethical decision framing (Tenbrunsel & Smith-Crowe, 2008) are largely synonymous with the term moral awareness in that they refer to processes whereby the decision-maker codes a decision as being relevant to morality, but unlike the term moral awareness, the terms moral construal and ethical decision framing do not imply a binary distinction between awareness versus unawareness (Tenbrunsel & Smith-Crowe, 2008). Moral attentiveness refers to a person’s chronic attention to moral issues in his/her everyday environment, and in defining this construct, Reynolds (2008) uses a lens metaphor. Unlike our theorizing about the character lens, Reynolds does not argue that a person’s moral lens is embedded in their moral character, but rather represents a stand-alone individual difference. Because the process of sensemaking is inherently integrative, our focus is on the holistic operation of moral recognition rather than on disentangling the specific nuanced facets that might comprise it.

  2. This is not to say that moral character influences interpretation of all decisions that individuals face in their daily lives—moral recognition is nonetheless still bounded by “reality constraints” (Kunda, 1990). Though a wide range of decisions can be moralized, at least for some people some of the time (Kreps & Monin, 2011), reality imposes some limits on what people can see in any given situation.

  3. Data collection occurred over the course of two academic years at both universities. The majority of the data (78.4%) were collected using the design described here. The remainder of data (1 year at one of the universities) were collected with fully hypothetical choices. This change did not substantively affect the results.

  4. Among these 281 participants, several had missing values for some items: round 3 moral recognition (n = 3), round 4 moral recognition (n = 2), round 2 internal whistleblowing (n = 2), round 1 external whistleblowing (n = 2), and round 3 external whistleblowing (n = 2). We used data from all 281 participants in our analyses and accounted for missing data by using the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation method.

References

  • Ameen, E. C., Guffey, D. M., & McMillan, J. J. (1996). Gender differences in determining the ethical sensitivity of future accounting professionals. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(5), 591–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aquino, K., Freeman, D., Reed, A., Lim, V. K. G., & Felps, W. (2009). Testing a social–cognitive model of moral behavior: The interactive influence of situations and moral identity centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 123–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aquino, K., & Reed, A. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1423–1440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2020). Objections to the HEXACO model of personality structure—And why those objections fail. European Journal of Personality, 34(4), 492–510. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, M. H., & Tenbrunsel, A. E. (2011). Blind spots: Why we fail to do what’s right and what to do about it. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, J. E., & Reynolds, W. M. (2016). Development, reliability, and validity of the Moral Identity Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 97, 120–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blodgett, J. G., Lu, L. C., Rose, G. M., & Vitell, S. J. (2001). Ethical sensitivity to stakeholder interests: A cross-cultural comparison. Academy of Marketing Science Journal, 29, 190–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, K. D., Treviño, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2000). Moral awareness in business organizations: Influences of issue-related and social context factors. Human Relations, 53(7), 981–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, A., Treviño, L. K., & Humphrey, S. E. (2020). Ethical champions, emotions, framing, and team ethical decision making. Journal of Applied Psychology, 105, 245–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 213–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Kim, Y., Jordan, K. P., & Panter, A. T. (2016). Guilt-proneness is a marker of integrity and employment suitability. Personality and Individual Differences, 92, 109–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Kim, Y., & Panter, A. T. (2014a). The five-item guilt proneness scale (GP-5). Carnegie Mellon University, https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G389S

  • Cohen, T. R., & Morse, L. (2014). Moral character: What it is and what it does. Research in Organizational Behavior, 34, 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., Turan, N., Morse, L., & Kim, Y. (2013). Agreement and similarity in self-other perceptions of moral character. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 816–830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.08.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., Turan, N., Morse, L. A., & Kim, Y. (2014b). Moral character in the workplace. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 943–963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 947–966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Helzer, E. G., & Creo, R. A. (in press). Honesty among lawyers: Moral character, game framing, and honest disclosures in negotiations. Negotiation Journal.

  • Crossan, M., Seijts, G., & Gandz, J. (2016). Developing leadership character. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desai, S. D., & Kouchaki, M. (2017). Moral symbols: A necklace of garlic against unethical requests. Academy of Management Journal, 60(1), 7–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, R. E. (2012). Personality predictors of leadership styles and the self-other agreement problem. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(5), 809–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2019). Why people trust: Solved puzzles and open mysteries. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 366–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, X., & Sivo, S. A. (2007). Sensitivity of fit indices to model misspecification and model types. Multivariate Behavior Research, 42(3), 509–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., Furr, R. M., Jayawickreme, E., Meindl, P., & Helzer, E. G. (2014). Character: The prospects for a personality-based perspective on morality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(4), 178–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (2015). Whole trait theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 82–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (2021). Whole traits: Revealing social cognitive mechanisms constituting personality’s central variable. In B. Gawronski (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 63, pp. 69–128). Elsevier.

  • Fleiss, J. L., Cohen, J., & Everitt, B. S. (1969). Large sample standard errors of kappa and weighted kappa. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 323–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gok, K., Sumanth, J., Bommer, W., Demirtas, O., Arslan, A., Eberhard, J., Özdemir, A., & Yigit, A. (2017). You may not reap what you sow: How employees’ moral awareness minimizes ethical leadership’s positive impact on workplace deviance. Journal of Business Ethics, 146, 257–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grover, S. L. (2014). Moral identity as a lens for interpreting honesty of indirect leaders. Journal of Change Management, 14, 48–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halevy, N., Chou, E. Y., & Murnighan, J. K. (2012). Mind games: The mental representation of conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 132–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halevy, N., & Katz, J. J. (2013). Conflict templates. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 217–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.

  • Heck, D. W., Thielmann, I., Moshagen, M., & Hilbig, B. (2018). Who lies? A large-scale re-analysis linking basic personality traits to unethical decision making. Judgement and Decision Making, 13(4), 356–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helzer, E. G., Furr, R. M., Hawkins, A., Barranti, M., Blackie, L. E. R., & Fleeson, W. (2014). Agreement on the perception of moral character. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12), 1698–1710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helzer, E. G., Furr, R. M., & Jayawickreme, E. (2018). Moral character: Current insights and future directions. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. Shackelford (Eds.), SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences. SAGE Publishing.

  • Hertz, S. G., & Krettenauer, T. (2016). Does moral identity effectively predict moral behavior? A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology, 20(2), 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2015). When the cat’s away, some mice will play: A basic trait account of dishonest behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 57, 72–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodson, G., Book, A., Visser, B. A., Volk, A. A., Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2018). Is the dark triad common factor distinct from low honesty-humility? Journal of Research in Personality, 73, 123–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jayawickreme, E., Meindl, P., Helzer, E. G., Furr, R. M., & Fleeson, W. (2014). Virtuous states and virtuous traits: How the empirical evidence regarding the existence of broad traits saves virtue ethics from the situationist critique. Theory and Research in Education, 12(3), 283–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research, (3rd ed., pp. 114–158). Guilford.

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. (2009). A social cognition framework for examining moral awareness in managers and academics. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(2), 237–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y., & Cohen, T. R. (2015). Moral character and workplace deviance: Recent research and current trends. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 134–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kochis, B. (2005). On lenses and filters: The role of metaphor in policy theory. Administrative Theory and Praxis, 27, 24–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kouchaki, M., Smith-Crowe, K., Brief, A. P., & Sousa, C. (2013). Seeing green: Mere exposure to money triggers a business decision frame and unethical outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(1), 53–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreps, T. A., & Monin, B. (2011). “Doing well by doing good”? Ambivalent moral framing in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 31, 99–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lapsley, D. (2016). Moral self-identity and the social–cognitive theory of virtue. In L. Annas, D. Narvaez, & N. Snow (Eds.), Advances in virtue development: Integrating perspectives (pp. 34–68). Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lapsley, D. K., & Lasky, B. (2001). Prototypic moral character. Identity: an International Journal of Theory and Research, 1, 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2012). The H factor of personality: Why some people are manipulative, self-entitled, materialistic, and exploitive—And why it matters for everyone. Wilfrid Laurier University Press.

  • Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Morrison, D. L., Cordery, J., & Dunlop, P. D. (2008). Predicting integrity with the HEXACO personality model: Use of self- and observer reports. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81, 147–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Pozzebon, J. A., Visser, B. A., Bourdage, J. S., & Ogunfowora, B. (2009). Similarity and assumed similarity in personality reports of well-acquainted persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 460–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, E. E., Bitterly, T. B., Cohen, T. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2018). Who is trustworthy? Predicting trustworthy intentions and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115, 468–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining prisoner’s dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1175–1185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, D. M., Aquino, K., Greenbaum, R. L., & Kuenzi, M. (2012). Who displays ethical leadership, and why does it matter? An examination of antecedents and consequences of ethical leadership. Academy of Management Journal, 55(1), 151–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., & Viswesvaran, C. (2005). Whistleblowing in organizations: An examination of correlates of whistleblowing intentions, actions, and retaliation. Journal of Business Ethics, 62, 277–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., & Gino, F. (2013). Ethically adrift: How others pull our moral compass from true North, and how we can fix it. Research in Organizational Behavior, 33, 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morse, L., & Cohen, T. R. (2019). Moral character in negotiation. Academy of Management Perspectives, 33(1), 12–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L., Krettenauer, T., & Narvaez, D. (Eds.) (2014). Handbook of moral and character education. Routledge.

  • Pillutla, M. M., Malhotra, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2003). Attributions of trust and the calculus of reciprocity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(5), 448–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, M. R., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Bazerman, M. H. (2019). Bounded ethicality and ethical fading in negotiations: Understanding unintended unethical behavior. Academy of Management Perspectives, 33(1), 26–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. Praeger.

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Moral awareness and ethical predispositions: Investigating the role of individual differences in the recognition of moral issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(1), 233–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2008). Moral attentiveness: Who pays attention to the moral aspects of life? Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(5), 1027–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., Leavitt, K., & DeCelles, K. A. (2010). Automatic ethics: The effects of implicit assumptions and contextual cues on moral behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(4), 752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., & Miller, J. A. (2015). The recognition of moral issues: Moral awareness, moral sensitivity and moral attentiveness. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 114–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solinger, O. N., Jansen, P. G., & Cornelissen, J. P. (2020). The emergence of moral leadership. Academy of Management Review, 45(3), 504–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonenshein, S. (2007). The role of construction, intuition, and justification in responding to ethical issues at work: The sensemaking-intuition model. Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1022–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, J. R. (2015). A social cognitive explanation of situational and individual effects on moral sensitivity. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45(1), 45–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, J. R., & Hunt, S. D. (1998). Marketing researcher ethical sensitivity: Concpetualization, measurement, and exploratory investigation. The Journal of Marketing, 62(2), 92–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (2004). Ethical fading: The role of self-deception in unethical behavior. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 223–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Smith-Crowe, K. (2008). Ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. The Academy of Management Annals, 2(1), 545–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). The traits one can trust: Dissecting reciprocity and kindness as determinants of trustworthy behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(11), 1523–1536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 30–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11(3), 601–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K., Weaver, G. R., Gibson, D. G., & Toffler, B. L. (1999). Managing ethics and legal compliance: What works and what hurts. California Management Review, 41, 131–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., Haas, I. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2012). The importance of moral construal: Moral versus non-moral construal elicits faster, more extreme, universal evaluations of the same actions. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e48693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weick, K. E. (1988). Enacted sensemaking in crisis situations. Journal of Management Studies, 25(4), 305–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weick, K. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, T., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2014). Morality rebooted: Exploring simple fixes to our moral bugs. Research in Organizational Behavior, 34, 63–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was made possible by funding provided by the Moral Beacon Project at Wake Forest University and the Templeton Religion Trust to Erik Helzer, Taya Cohen, and Brandy Aven. The opinions expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the sponsors, the US government, or Department of Defense. We thank Brandy Aven, Alessandro Iorio, Kori Krueger, Laurie Weingart, and attendees of the Moral Beacon Project meetings for their help with the development and refinement of this research, as well as Steve Carney and the Center for Behavioral and Decision Research at Carnegie Mellon University for providing research assistance. Portions of this work were presented at the 2018 meeting of the Academy of Management. Study materials and data files for this research are available on the Open Science Framework: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/A2YGJ.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erik G. Helzer.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Helzer, E.G., Cohen, T.R. & Kim, Y. The Character Lens: A Person-Centered Perspective on Moral Recognition and Ethical Decision-Making. J Bus Ethics 182, 483–500 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-021-05010-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-021-05010-z

Keywords

Navigation