Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 28, Issue 5, pp 853–871

Morgan’s Canon, meet Hume’s Dictum: avoiding anthropofabulation in cross-species comparisons

Article

Abstract

How should we determine the distribution of psychological traits—such as Theory of Mind, episodic memory, and metacognition—throughout the Animal kingdom? Researchers have long worried about the distorting effects of anthropomorphic bias on this comparative project. A purported corrective against this bias was offered as a cornerstone of comparative psychology by C. Lloyd Morgan in his famous “Canon”. Also dangerous, however, is a distinct bias that loads the deck against animal mentality: our tendency to tie the competence criteria for cognitive capacities to an exaggerated sense of typical human performance. I dub this error “anthropofabulation”, since it combines anthropocentrism with confabulation about our own prowess. Anthropofabulation has long distorted the debate about animal minds, but it is a bias that has been little discussed and against which the Canon provides no protection. Luckily, there is a venerable corrective against anthropofabulation: a principle offered long ago by David Hume, which I call “Hume’s Dictum”. In this paper, I argue that Hume’s Dictum deserves a privileged place next to Morgan’s Canon in the methodology of comparative psychology, illustrating my point through a discussion of the debate over Theory of Mind in nonhuman animals.

Keywords

Anthropocentrism Anthropomorphism Comparative psychology Morgan’s Canon Bias 

References

  1. Andrews K (2005) Chimpanzee theory of mind: looking in all the wrong places? Mind Lang 20(5):521–536CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews K (2012) Do apes read minds? Toward a new folk psychology. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  3. Apperly I, Buterfill S (2009) Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychol Rev 116:953–970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ariely D (2009) Predictably irrational: the hidden forces that shape our decisions. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Ariely D (2012) The (honest) truth about dishonesty: how we lie to everyone–especially ourselves. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Barrett L (2008) Out of their heads: turning relational reinterpretation inside out. Behav Brain Sci 31(2):130–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bermúdez JL (2003) The domain of folk psychology. In: O’Hear A (ed) Minds and persons. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 1–29Google Scholar
  8. Boesch C (2007) What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. J Comp Psychol 121(3):227–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boyd R (1999) Homeostasis, species, and higher taxa. In: Wilson R (ed) Species: new interdisciplinary essays. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 141–185Google Scholar
  10. Buckner C (2013) The semantic problem(s) with research on animal mindreading. Mind LangGoogle Scholar
  11. Buckner R, Carroll D (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11(2):49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bugnyar T (2007) An integrative approach to the study of “theory-of-mind”-like abilities in ravens. Jpn J Anim Psychol 57(1):15–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bulloch M, Boysen S, Furlong E (2008) Visual attention and its relation to knowledge states in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim Behav 76(4):1147–1155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn Sci 12(5):187–192. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carruthers P (2008) Meta-cognition in animals: a skeptical look. Mind Lang 23(1):58–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carruthers P (2009) How we know our own minds: the relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behav Brain Sci 32(2):121–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De Waal F (2000) Anthropomorphism and anthropodenial: consistency in our thinking about humans and other animals. Philos Top 27:255–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dretske F (1986) Misrepresentation. In: Bogan R (ed) Belief: form, content and function. New York, Oxford, pp 17–36Google Scholar
  19. Dretske F (1988) Explaining behavior: reasons in a world of causes. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  20. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2009) Comparative social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 60(1):87–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fitzpatrick S (2008) Doing away with Morgan’s canon. Mind Lang 23(2):224–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fitzpatrick S (2009) The primate mindreading controversy: a case study in simplicity and methodology in animal psychology. In: Lurz R (ed) The philosophy of animal minds. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 258–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fletcher L, Carruthers P (2012) Behavior–reading versus mentalizing in animals. In: Metcalfe J, Terrace H (eds) Agency and Joint attention. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  24. Flombaum JI, Santos LR (2005) Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Curr Biol 15(5):447–452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gallagher S, Povinelli D (2012) Enactive and behavioral abstraction accounts of social understanding in chimpanzees, infants, and adults. Rev Philos Psychol 3:145–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (2002) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgement. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Griffiths P (1999) Squaring the circle: natural kinds with historical essences. In: Wilson R (ed) Species: new interdisciplinary essays. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 208–228Google Scholar
  28. Heyes C (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21:101–148Google Scholar
  29. Hume D (1739) A treatise on human nature. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  30. Keeley B (2004) Anthropomorphism, primatomorphism, mammalomorphism: understanding cross-species comparisons. Biol Philos 19:521–540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lurz R (2009) If chimpanzees are mindreaders, could behavioral science tell? Toward a solution of the logical problem. Philos Psychol 22(3):305–328. doi:10.1080/09515080902970673 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lurz R (2011) Mindreading animals: the debate over what animals know about other minds. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  33. Macmillan N (2002) Signal detection theory. In: Yantis S (ed) Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology. Coughlan Publishing, MankatoGoogle Scholar
  34. Malle B (2011) Time to give up the dogmas of attribution: an alternative theory of behavior explanation. In: Zanna M (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology. Academic Press, Salt Lake CityGoogle Scholar
  35. Malle B, Knobe J, Nelson S (2007) Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: new answers to an old question. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(4):491–514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McGonigle B, Chalmers M (2008) Putting Descartes before the horse (again!). Behav Brain Sci 32(2):142–143Google Scholar
  37. Morgan CL (1903) An introduction to comparative psychology. In: Scott W (ed), p 386Google Scholar
  38. Morgan CL (1930) Autobiography of C. Lloyd Morgan. In: Murchison C (ed) History of psychology in autobiography, vol 2. Clark University Press, Worcester, MA, pp 237–264Google Scholar
  39. Nisbett R, Ross L (1980) Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice-Hall, Englewood CliffsGoogle Scholar
  40. Nisbett R, Ross L (1991) The person and the situation. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Nisbett R, Wilson T (1977) Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev 84(3):231–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Penn D, Povinelli D (2007) On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a theory of mind. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 362(1480):731–744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Penn D, Povinelli D (2013) The comparative delusion: the behaviorstic/mentalistic dichotomy in comparative theory of mind research. Terrace HA, Metcalfe J (eds) Agency and joint attention, Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  44. Penn D, Holyoak K, Povinelli D (2008) Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. In: Smith B, Woodruff Smith D (eds) Behav Brain Sci, 31(2): 109–130; discussion 130–178Google Scholar
  45. Piolino P, Hisland M, Ruffeveille I, Matuszewski V, Jambaque I, Eustache F (2007) Do school-age children remember or know the personal past? Conscious Cogn 16(2007):84–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Povinelli D (2004) Behind the ape’s appearance: escaping anthropocentrism in the study of other minds. Daedalus 133(1):29–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Povinelli D (2011) World without weight: perspectives on an alien mind. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Povinelli D, Eddy T (1996) What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 61(3):1–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Povinelli D, Vonk J (2003) Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends Cogn Sci 7(4):157–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Povinelli D, Vonk J (2004) We don’t need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee’s mind. Mind Lang 19(1):1–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1(04):515–526. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Radick G (2000) Morgan’s canon, Garner’s phonograph, and the evolutionary origins of language and reason. Br J Hist Sci 33(1):3–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Richards R (1989) Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary theories of mind and behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  54. Santos L, Flombaum J, Phillips W (2007) The evolution of human mindreading: how non-human primates can inform social cognitive neuroscience. In: Platek S, Keenan J, Shackleford T (eds) Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 433–456Google Scholar
  55. Schacter D (1999) The seven sins of memory: insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Am Psychol 54(3):182–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schnider A (2008) The confabulating mind: how the brain creates reality. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Shettleworth S (2009) The evolution of comparative cognition: is the snark still a boojum? Behav Process 80(3):210–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Simons D, Chabris C (2011) What people believe about how memory works: a representative survey of the US population. PLoS ONE 6(8):e22757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sober E (1998) Morgan’s canon. In: Cummins D, Allen C (eds) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 224–242Google Scholar
  60. Sober E (2005) Comparative psychology meets evolutionary biology: Morgan’s canon and cladistic parsimony. In: Dalston L, Mitman G (eds) Thinking with animals: new perspectives on anthropomorphism. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  61. Suddendorf T, Corballis M (2008) New evidence for animal foresight? Anim Behav 75(5):e1–e3. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Thomas R (2006) Lloyd Morgan’s Canon: a history of misrepresentation. Hist Theory Psychol Epr ArchGoogle Scholar
  63. Tomasello M, Call J (2006) Do chimpanzees know what others see—or only what they are looking at? In: Hurley S, Nudds M (eds) Rational animals? Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  64. Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (2003) Chimpanzees versus humans: it’s not that simple. Trends Cogn Sci 7(8):239–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Tulving E (1985) Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol 26(1):1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185(4157):1124–1131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Van Hippel W, Trivers R (2011) The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behav Brain Sci 34(1):1–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wynne C (2007) What are animals? Why anthropomorphism is still not a scientific approach to behavior. Comp Cogn Behav Rev 2:125–135Google Scholar
  69. Ylikoski P, Kuorikoski J (2010) Dissecting explanatory power. Philos Stud 148(2):201–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of HoustonHoustonUSA
  2. 2.Ruhr-University BochumBochumGermany

Personalised recommendations