How do you measure pleasure? A discussion about intrinsic costs and benefits in primate allogrooming
- 527 Downloads
- 12 Citations
Abstract
Social grooming is an important element of social life in terrestrial primates, inducing the putative benefits of β-endorphin stimulation and group harmony and cohesion. Implicit in many analyses of grooming (e.g. biological markets) are the assumptions of costs and benefits to grooming behaviour. Here, in a review of literature, we investigate the proximate costs and benefits of grooming, as a potentially useful explanatory substrate to the well-documented ultimate (functional) explanations. We find that the hedonic benefits of grooming are well documented. However, we did not find convincing evidence for costs. If proximate costs do exist, they might consist of energetic, cognitive, opportunity costs, or some combination of all of these. Nonetheless, there remains the possibility that grooming costs are negligible, or even that the provision of allogrooming is rewarding in itself. We suggest empirical research to resolve this issue.
Keywords
Grooming Value Sociality Primates Cost Benefit Game theoryNotes
Acknowledgments
Yvan Russell was partly funded by the German Initiative of Excellence of the German Science Foundation (DFG).
References
- Angel RW, Quick WM, Boylls CC, Weinrich M, Rodnitzky RL (1985) Decrement of somatosensory evoked potentials during repetitive stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuropsychol 60:335–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Aureli F, Preston SD, de Waal FBM (1999) Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. J Comp Psychol 113:59–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Axelrod R (1997) The complexity of cooperation: agent-based models of competition and collaboration. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
- Barrett L, Henzi SP (2001) The utility of grooming in baboon troops. In: Noë R, van Hooff JARAM, Hammerstein P (eds) Economics in nature: social dilemmas, mate choice and biological markets. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 119–145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (1999) Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 266:665–670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (2000) Female baboons do not raise the stakes but they give as good as they get. Anim Behav 59:763–770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Binmore K (2007) Playing for real: a text on game theory. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Boccia ML (1983) A functional analysis of social grooming patterns through direct comparison with self-grooming in Rhesus Monkeys. Int J Primatol 4:399–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000) Chimpanzees of the Taï forest. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
- Bräuer J, Hanus D (2012) Fairness in non human primates? Soc Justice Res 25:256–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Busemeyer JR, Johnson JG (2005) Micro-process models of decision making. In: Sun R (ed) Cambridge handbook of computational psychology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 302–321Google Scholar
- Cady SH, Jones GE (1997) Massage therapy as a workplace intervention for reduction of stress. Percept Mot Skills 84:157–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Waal FBM (1997) The chimpanzee’s service economy: food for grooming. Evol Hum Behav 18:375–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dugatkin LA (1997) Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Croon Helm, LondonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM (1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol 57:121–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM (1992) Time: a hidden constraint in the socioecology of baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 31:35–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM (2010) The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:260–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM, Sharman M (1984) Is social grooming altruistic? Z Tierpsychol 64:163–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM, Shultz S (2010) Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour 147:775–803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunbar RIM, Korstjens AH, Lehmann J (2009) Time as an ecological constraint. Biol Rev 84:413–429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Feh C, de Mazières J (1993) Grooming at a preferred site reduces heart rate in horses. Anim Behav 46:1191–1194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Feldman F (1997) On the intrinsic value of pleasures. Ethics 107:448–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F (2008) Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad USA 105:8557–8562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goats GC (1994) Massage—the scientific basis of an ancient art part 2. Physiological and therapeutic effects. Br J Sports Med 28:153–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gobet F, Chassy P, Bilalić M (2011) Foundations of cognitive psychology. McGraw Hill, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
- Graves FC, Wallen K, Maestripieri D (2002) Opioids and attachment in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) abusive mothers. Behav Neurosci 116:489–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Grueter CC, Bisonette A, Isler K, van Schaik CP (2013) Grooming and group cohesion in primates: implications for the evolution of language. Evol Hum Behav 34:61–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hawkes C (1992) Endorphins: the basis of pleasure? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:247–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hensher DA, Johnson LW (1981) Applied discrete-choice modelling. Croon Helm, LondonGoogle Scholar
- Higgins ET (2006) Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychol Rev 113:439–460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hinde RA (1976) Interactions, relationships, and social structure. Man 11:1–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hutchins M, Barash DP (1976) Grooming in primates: implications for its utilitarian function. Primates 17:145–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kaada B, Torsteinbø O (1989) Increase of plasma β-endorphins in connective tissue massage. Gen Pharmacol 20:487–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B (1989) Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 14:155–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koyama NF, Caws C, Aureli F (2006) Interchange of grooming and agonistic support in chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 27:1293–1309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mindreading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
- Kropp P, Linstedt U, Niederberger U, Gerber WD (2001) Contingent negative variation and attentional performance in humans. Neurol Res 23:647–650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lane PCR, Gobet F, Smith RL (2008) Attention mechanisms in the CHREST cognitive architecture. In: Paletta L, Tsotsos JK (eds) Proceedings of the 5th international workshop on attention in cognitive systems. Joanneum Research, Graz, pp 207–220Google Scholar
- McBride SD, Hemmings A, Robinson K (2004) A preliminary study on the effect of massage to reduce stress in the horse. J Equine Vet Sci 24:76–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McSweeney FK, Swindell S (1999) General-process theories of motivation revisited: the role of habituation. Psychol Bull 125:437–457CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mooring MS, Hart BL (1995) Costs of allogrooming in impala: distraction from vigilance. Anim Behav 49:1414–1416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nagai Y, Critchley HD, Featherstone E, Fenwick PBC, Trimble MR, Dolan RJ (2004) Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 21:1232–1241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Naish P (2010) Attention. In: Kaye H (ed) Cognitive psychology, 2nd edn. The Open University, Milton KeynesGoogle Scholar
- Newton-Fisher NE (1999) Association by male chimpanzees: a social tactic? Behaviour 136:705–730CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nishida T (1970) Social behavior and relationship among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali mountains. Primates 11:47–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nishida T, Kano T, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nakamura M (1999) Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropol Sci 107:141–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Noë R, Hammerstein P (1995) Biological markets. Trends Ecol Evol 10:336–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Noë R, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM (1991) The market effect: an explanation for payoff asymmetries among collaborating animals. Ethology 87:97–118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nowak A, Sigmund K (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437:1291–1298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Parker GA, Maynard Smith J (1990) Optimality theory in evolutionary biology. Nature 348:27–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pérez AP, Veà JJ (2000) Allogrooming behavior in Cercocebus torquatus: the case for the hygienic functional hypothesis. Primates 41:199–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pérez A, Vèa JJ (1998) Cost-benefit analysis of allogrooming behaviour in primates II. Primate Rep 50:15–37Google Scholar
- Puga-Gonzalez I, Hildenbrandt H, Hemelrijk CK (2009) Emergent patterns of social affiliation in primates, a model. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000630CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Roberts WA (2002) Are animals stuck in time? Psychol Bull 3:473–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Russell YI (2007) Reputations and polyadic interactions among great apes. Ph.D. thesis, University of LiverpoolGoogle Scholar
- Russell YI (2010) Third party grooming in a captive chimpanzee group. Primates 51:79–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Russell YI, Call J, Dunbar RIM (2008) Image scoring in great apes. Behav Process 78:108–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sato S, Tarumizu K (1993) Heart rates before, during and after allo-grooming in cattle (Bos taurus). J Ethol 11:149–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schino G, Aureli F (2009) Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition, and emotions. Adv Study Behav 39:45–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schino G, Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Turillazzi PG (1988) Allogrooming as a tension reduction mechanism: a behavioral approach. Am J Primatol 16:43–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302:1231–1234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stammbach E (1988) Group responses to specially skilled individuals in a Macaca fascicularis group. Behaviour 107:241–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tanaka I (1998) Social diffusion of modified louse egg-handling techniques during grooming in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Anim Behav 56:1229–1236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tanaka I, Takefushi H (1993) Elimination of external parasites (lice) is the primary function of grooming in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Anthropol Sci 101:187–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol 20:410–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Touge T, Ikeguchi M, Deguchi K, Watanabe S, Kuriyama S, Takeuchi H (2003) Effects of serial visuomotor tasks on contingent negative variation. Int J Neurosci 113:431–443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van Lawick-Goodall J (1968) The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim Behav Monogr 1:161–311Google Scholar
- van Ree JM, Niesink RJM (1983) Low doses of β-endorphin increase social contacts of rats tested in dyadic encounters. Life Sci 33:S611–S614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van Ree JM, Niesink RJM, van Wolfswinkel L, Ramsey NF, Kornet MLMW, van Furth WR, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Gerrits MAFM, van den Berg CL (2000) Endogenous opioids and reward. Eur J Pharmacol 405:89–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Veà JJ, Pérez AP, Baldellou M, Alea V (1999) Cost-benefit analysis of allogrooming behaviour in Cerococebus torquatus lunulatus. Behaviour 136:243–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ventura R, Majolo B, Koyama NF, Hardie S, Schino G (2006) Reciprocation and interchange in wild Japanese macaques: grooming, cofeeding, and agonistic support. Am J Primatol 68:1138–1149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Videan EN, Fritz J, Schwandt M, Howell S (2005) Neighbor effect: evidence of affiliative and agonistic social contagion in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Primatol 66:131–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldridge VJ, McCallum WC, Winter AL (1964) Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 203:380–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Watts DP (2002) Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour 139:343–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Watts DP (2003) Interchange of grooming and coalitionary support by wild male chimpanzees. Am J Phys Anthropol 117:S162Google Scholar
- Yaksh TL, Gross KE, Li CH (1982) Studies on the intrathecal effect of β-endorphin in primate. Brain Res 241:261–269CrossRefGoogle Scholar