Advertisement

Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 28, Issue 3, pp 457–479 | Cite as

Culture in humans and other animals

  • Grant RamseyEmail author
Article

Abstract

The study of animal culture is a flourishing field, with culture being recorded in a wide range of taxa, including non-human primates, birds, cetaceans, and rodents. In spite of this research, however, the concept of culture itself remains elusive. There is no universally assented to concept of culture, and there is debate over the connection between culture and related concepts like tradition and social learning. Furthermore, it is not clear whether culture in humans and culture in non-human animals is really the same thing, or merely loose analogues that go by the same name. The purpose of this paper is to explicate core desiderata for a concept of culture and then to construct a concept that meets these desiderata. The paper then applies this concept in both humans and non-human animals.

Keywords

Behavior Culture Epigenetic Evolution Innovation Social learning Tradition 

Notes

Acknowledgments

I thank Robert Brandon, Andreas De Block, Daniel Dennett, Agustín Fuentes, Dan McShea, Güven Güzeldere, Maya Parson, Charles Pence, Alex Rosenberg, Kim Sterelny, and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable input into this paper.

References

  1. Acerbi A, Tennie C, Nunn CL (2011) Modeling imitation and emulation in constrained search spaces. Learn Behav 39(2):104–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alvard MS (2003) The adaptive nature of culture. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 12(3):136–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anway MD, Cupp AS, Uzumcu M, Skinner MK (2005) Epigenetic transgenerational actions of endocrine disruptors and male fertility. Science 308:1466–1469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Appadurai A (1996) Modernity at large: cultural dimensions of globalization, vol 1. University of Minnesota Press, MinneapolisGoogle Scholar
  5. Aunger R (1999) Culture as consensus: against idealism/contra consensus; cultural consensus as a statistical model. Curr Anthropol 40(S1):93–101Google Scholar
  6. Boesch C, Tomasello M (1998) Chimpanzee and human cultures. Curr Anthropol 39(5):591–614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boesch C, Marchesi P, Marchesi N, Fruth B, Joulian F (1994) Is nutcracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? J Hum Evol 26:325–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonner JT (1980) The evolution of culture in animals. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
  9. Bourdieu P (1990) In other words: essays towards a reflexive sociology. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto, CAGoogle Scholar
  10. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. The University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  11. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  12. Brumann C (1999) Writing for culture. Curr Anthropol 40(S1):1–27Google Scholar
  13. Byrne RW, Barnard PJ, Davidson I, Janik VM, McGrew WC, Miklósi A, Wiessner P (2004) Understanding culture across species. Trends Cogn Sci 8(8):341–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Caldwell CA, Millen AE (2009) Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution is imitation necessary? Psychol Sci 20(12):1478–1483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Caldwell CA, Millen AE (2010) Human cumulative culture in the laboratory: effects of (Micro) population size. Learn Behav 38(3):310–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Caldwell CA, Whiten A (2011) Social learning in monkeys and apes: cultural animals? In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M (eds) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 652–662Google Scholar
  17. Caldwell CA, Schillinger K, Evans CL, Hopper LM (2012) End state copying by Humans (Homo Sapiens): implications for a comparative perspective on cumulative culture. J Comp Psychol 126(2):161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Call J, Carpenter M (2002) Three sources of information in social learning. In: Dautenhahn K, Nehaniv CL (eds) Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 211–228Google Scholar
  19. Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 Years later. Trends Cogn Sci 12(5):187–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Call J, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2005) Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo Sapiens). Animal Cogn 8(3):151–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  22. Clifford J (1988) The predicament of culture: twentieth-century ethnography, literature, and art. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  23. Cronk L (1999) That complex whole. Westview Press, BoulderGoogle Scholar
  24. Csibra G, Gergely G (2006) Social learning and social cognition: the case for pedagogy. Process Change Brain Cogn Dev Atten Perform XXI:249–274Google Scholar
  25. Csibra G, Gergely G (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1149–1157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Darwin Charles (1859) On the origin of species by means of natural selection. Murray, LondonGoogle Scholar
  27. Dawkins R (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  28. de Waal FBM, Bonnie KE (2009) In tune with others: the social side of primate culture. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 19–40Google Scholar
  29. Dretske F (1983) Knowledge and the flow of information. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  30. Durham WH (1991) Coevolution. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto, CAGoogle Scholar
  31. Ehn M, Laland K (2012) Adaptive strategies for cumulative cultural learning. J Theor Biol 301:103–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Enquist M, Ghirlanda S (2007) Evolution of social learning does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture. J Theor Biol 246(1):129–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Flinn MV (1997) Culture and the evolution of social learning. Evol Human Behav 18(1):23–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Flinn MV, Alexander RD (1982) Culture theory: the developing synthesis from biology. Human Ecol 10(3):383–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Fox RG (1999) Culture—a second chance? Curr Anthropol 40(S1):Si–SiiGoogle Scholar
  36. Fragaszy DM, Perry S (2003) The biology of traditions: models and evidence. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  37. Friedman J (1994) Cultural identity and global process, vol 31. Sage Publications Ltd, Thousand OaksGoogle Scholar
  38. Galef BG (1992) The question of animal culture. Human Nat 3:157–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Galef BG Jr (1996) Social enhancement of food preferences in Norway rats: a brief review. In: Heyes C, Galef BG Jr (eds) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic Press, New York, pp 49–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Galef BG, Sherry DF (1973) Mother’s milk: a medium for transmission of cues reflecting the flavor of mother’s diet. J Comp Physiol Psychol 83:374–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Geertz C (1973) The interpretation of cultures. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  42. Gergely G, Csibra G (2005) The social construction of the cultural mind: imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interact Stud 6(3):463–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Enfield NJ, Levenson SC (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition, and human interaction. Berg Publishers, Oxford, pp 229–255Google Scholar
  44. Gissis SB, Jablonka E (2011) Transformations of lamarckism: from subtle fluids to molecular biology. The MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  45. Gruber T, Muller MN, Strimling P, Wrangham R, Zuberbuhler K (2009) Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Curr Biol 19(21):1806–1810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Hart H, Pantzer A (1925) Have subhuman animals culture? Am J Sociol 30:703–709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Hepper PG (1988) Adaptive fetal learning: prenatal exposure to garlic affects postnatal preferences. Anim Behav 36(3):935–936CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Herrmann E, Call J, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M (2007) Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317(5843):1360–1366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Heyes CM (1993) Imitation, culture and cognition. Animal Behav 46(5):999–1010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Heyes CM (1994) Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev 69(2):207–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hopper LM, Spiteri A, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Horner V, Whiten A (2007) Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees. Anim Behav 73(6):1021–1032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hopper LM, Schapiro SJ, Lambeth SP, Brosnan SF (2011) Chimpanzees’ socially maintained food preferences indicate both conservatism and conformity. Animal Behav 81(6):1195–1202Google Scholar
  53. Hopper LM, Marshall-Pescini S, Whiten A (2012) Social learning and culture in child and chimpanzee. In: De Waal FBM, Ferrari PF (eds) The primate mind: built to connect with other minds. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, 99–118Google Scholar
  54. Jablonka E, Lamb M (2005) Evolution in four dimensions—genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  55. Kaffman A, Meaney MJ (2007) Neurodevelopmental sequelae of postnatal maternal care in rodents: clinical and research implications of molecular insights. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48:224–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Keesing RM (1994) Theories of culture revisited. In: Borofsky R (ed) Assessing cultural anthropology. McGraw-Hill Inc, New York, pp 301–310Google Scholar
  57. Kroeber AL, Kluckhohn C (1952) Culture: a critical review of concepts and definitions. The Museum, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  58. Kuper A (1999) Culture: an anthropologists’ account. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  59. Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32(1):4–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Laland KN, Galef BG (2009) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  61. Laland KN, Hoppitt W (2003) Do animals have culture? Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 12(3):150–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Lamarck JB (1809) Philosophie zoologique. C. Martins, ParisGoogle Scholar
  63. Linton R (1936) The study of man. Appelton-Century Crofts, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  64. Lyons DE, Damrosch DH, Lin JK, Macris DM, Keil FC (2011) The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1158–1167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Marshall-Pescini S, Whiten A (2008) Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach. Animal Cogn 11(3):449–456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. McGrew WC (2009) Ten dispatches from the chimpanzee culture wars, plus postscript (revisiting the battlefronts). In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 41–69Google Scholar
  67. McGrew WC, Tutin CEG (1978) Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man 13(2):234–251Google Scholar
  68. Meltzoff AN (1988) Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Dev Psychol 24(4):470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Mersmann D, Tomasello M, Call J, Kaminski J, Taborsky M (2011) Simple mechanisms can explain social learning in domestic dogs (Canis Familiaris). Ethology 117:1–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Moffatt B (2011) Conflations in the causal account of information undermine the parity thesis. Philos Sci 78:284–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Mundinger PC (1980) Animal cultures and a general theory of cultural evolution. Ethol Sociobiol 1(3):183–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Nielsen M (2012) Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture? J Comp Psychol 126(2):170–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  74. Over H, Carpenter M (2012) Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. J Comp Psychol 126(2):182–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Pembrey ME, Bygren LO, Kaati G, Edvinsson S, Northstone K, Sjöström M, Golding J, The ALSPAC Study Team (2006) Sex-specific, male-line transgenerational responses in humans. Eur J Human Genet 14:159–166Google Scholar
  76. Premack D, Houser MD (2006) Why animals do not have culture. In: Levinson SC, Jaisson P (eds) Evolution and culture. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  77. Quiring R, Walldorf U, Kloter U, Gehring WJ (1994) Homology of the eyeless gene of drosophila to the small eye gene in mice and aniridia in humans. Science 265:785–789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Ramsey G (2007) The fundamental constraint on the evolution of culture. Biol Philos 22:401–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Ramsey G, Peterson AS (2012) Sameness in biology. Philos Sci 79(2):255–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Ramsey G, Bastian ML, van Schaik C (2007) Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behav Brain Sci 30:393–437Google Scholar
  81. Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. The University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  82. Sapir E (1924) Culture, genuine and spurious. Am J Sociol 29:401–429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Sargeant BL, Mann J (2009) From social learning to culture: Intrapopulation variation in bottlenose dolphins. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 152–173Google Scholar
  84. Schneider D (1976) Notes toward a theory of culture. In: Basso KH, Selby HA (eds) Meaning and anthropology. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque, 197–220Google Scholar
  85. Shannon CE (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J 27:379–423Google Scholar
  86. Shannon CE, Weaver W (1949) The mathematical theory of communication. University of Illinois Press, UrbanaGoogle Scholar
  87. Shubin Neil, Tabin Cliff, Carroll Sean (2009) Deep homology and the origins of evolutionary novelty. Nature 457:818–823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Shweder RA (2001) Rethinking the object of anthropology and ending up where Kroeber and Kluckhohn began. Am Anthropol 103(2):437–440Google Scholar
  89. Sterelny K (2009) Peacekeeping in the culture wars. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 288–304Google Scholar
  90. Sterelny K (2012) The evolved apprentice. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  91. Strauss C, Quinn N (1997) A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  92. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364(1528):2405–2415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Tomasello M (1990) Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In: Parker S, Gibson K (eds) ‘Language’ and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 274–311Google Scholar
  94. Tomasello M (1996) Do apes ape? In: Heyes CM, Galef BG Jr (eds) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, USA, pp 319–346Google Scholar
  95. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(5):675–690Google Scholar
  96. Tyler EB (1871) Primitive culture. Brentano’s, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  97. van Schaik CP, Ancrenaz M, Borgen G, Galdikas B, Knott CD, Singleton I, Suzuki A, Utami SS, Merrill MY (2003) Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299:102–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Weaver ICG, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D’Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, Dymov S, Szyf M, Meaney MJ (2004) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci 7:847–854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Whiten A (2009) The identification and differentiation of culturein chimpanzees and other animals: from natural history to diffusion experiments. In: Laland KN, Galef BG (eds) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 99–124Google Scholar
  100. Whiten A, Ham R (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. Adv Study Behav 21:239–283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Whiten A, van Schaik CP (2007) The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Phil Trans R Soc B 362(1480):603–620Google Scholar
  102. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Whiten A, Horner V, Marshall-Pescini S (2003) Cultural panthropology. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 12(2):92–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Whiten A, McGuigan N, Marshall-Pescini S, Hopper LM (2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364(1528):2417–2428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Willey EE (1929) The validity of the culture concept. Am J Sociol 35:204–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Wissler C (1929) Introduction to social anthropology. Henry Holt and Co, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of Notre DameNotre DameUSA

Personalised recommendations