Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 27, Issue 6, pp 811–831 | Cite as

An evolutionary perspective on the long-term efficiency of costly punishment

Article

Abstract

Many studies show that punishment, although able to stabilize cooperation at high levels, destroys gains which makes it less efficient than alternatives with no punishment. Standard public goods games (PGGs) in fact show exactly these patterns. However, both evolutionary theory and real world institutions give reason to expect institutions with punishment to be more efficient, particularly in the long run. Long-term cooperative partnerships with punishment threats for non-cooperation should outperform defection prone non-punishing ones. This article demonstrates that fieldwork data from hunter-gatherers, common pool resource management cases and even PGGs support this hypothesis. Although earnings in PGGs with a punishment option may be lower at the beginning, efficiency increases dramatically over time. Most ten-period PGGs cannot capture this change because their time horizon is too short.

Keywords

Efficiency Punishment Public goods games Cooperation Hunter-gatherer Evolution 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The generosity of James Walker, Martin Sefton and Robert Schupp; Ernst Fehr; Nikos Nikiforakis and Hans-Theo Normann; Simon Gächter and Elke Renner to make their data publicly available or allowing me to analyze them is very much appreciated. Thanks to Eckart Voland and an anonymous reviewer for helpful discussions.

References

  1. Aktipis CA (2011) Is cooperation viable in mobile organisms? Simple Walk Away rule favors the evolution of cooperation in groups. Evolut Hum Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.01.002
  2. Andreoni J (1988) Why free ride? Strategies and learning in public goods experiments. J Public Econom 37(3):291–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Axelrod R (1984/2000) Die Evolution der Kooperation. Oldenbourg, MünchenGoogle Scholar
  4. Binmore K (2006) Why do people cooperate? J Polit Philos Econ 5(1):81–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bochet O, Page T, Putterman L (2006) Communication and punishment in contribution experiments. J Econ Behav Organ 60(1):11–26. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2003.06.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowles S (2006) Group competition, reproductive leveling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314(5805):1569–1572. doi:10.1126/science.1134829 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowles S (2009) Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science 324(5932):1293–1298. doi:10.1126/science.1168112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proc Nat Acad Sci 100(6):3531–3535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brockhurst MA, Buckling A, Racey D, Gardner A (2008) Resource supply and the evolution of public-goods cooperation in bacteria. BMC Biol 6:20–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Burnham TC, Johnson DDP (2005) The biological and evolutionary logic of human cooperation. Analyse und Kritik 27:113–135Google Scholar
  11. Buss DM (2004) Evolutionary psychology: the new science of the mind. Pearson, BostonGoogle Scholar
  12. Cardenas S (2000) How do groups solve local commons dilemmas? Lessons from experimental economics in the field. Environ Dev Sustain 2(3–4):305–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cardenas J (2003) Real wealth and experimental cooperation: experiments in the field lab. J Dev Econ 70:263–289. doi:10.1016/S0304-3878(02)00098-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Casari M (2003) Decentralized management of common property resources: experiments with a centuries-old institution. J Econ Behav Organ 51(2):217–247. doi:10.1016/S0167-2681(02)00098-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chaudhuri A (2011) Sustaining cooperation in laboratory public goods experiments: a selective survey of the literature. Exp Econ 14(1):47–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chhatre A, Agrawal A (2008) Forest commons and local enforcement. Proc Nat Acad Sci 105(36):13286–13291. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803399105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA (1995) Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373(6511):209–216. doi:10.1038/373209a0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dreber A, Rand DG, Fudenberg D, Nowak MA, Rand DG, Nowak MA (2008) Winners don’t punish. Nature 452(7185):348–351. doi:10.1038/nature06723 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Egas M, Riedl A (2008) The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 275:871–878. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1558 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ember CR (1978) Myths about Hunter-Gatherers. Ethnology 17(4):439–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425(6960):785–791. doi:10.1038/nature02043 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fehr E, Gächter S (2000) Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. Am Econ Rev 90(4):980–994CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fehr E, Gächter S (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415(6868):137–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Frey U (2007) Der blinde Fleck—Kognitive Fehler in der Wissenschaft und ihre evolutionsbiologischen Grundlagen. Ontos, HeusenstammGoogle Scholar
  25. Gächter S, Herrmann B (2009) Reciprocity, culture and human cooperation: previous insights and a new cross-cultural experiment. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1518):791–806. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0275 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gächter S, Renner E, Sefton M (2008) The long-run benefits of punishment. Science 322:1510. doi:10.1126/science.1164744 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gibson CC, Williams JT, Ostrom E (2005) Local enforcement and better forests. World Dev 33(2):273–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gigerenzer G, Todd PM, ABC Research Group (eds) (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  29. Gintis H (2009) The bounds of reason. Game theory and the unification of the behavioral sciences. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  30. Guala F (2012) Reciprocity: weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behav Brain Sci 35(1):1–15. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gürerk Ö, Irlenbusch B, Rockenbach B (2006) The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312(5770):108–111. doi:10.1126/science.1123633 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gurven M (2004) To give and to give not: the behavioral ecology of human food transfers. Behav Brain Sci 27(4):543–583. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04000123 Google Scholar
  33. Hamilton WD (1964a) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. J Theor Biol 7:1–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hamilton WD (1964b) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. J Theor Biol 7:17–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Haselton MG, Bryant GA, Wilke A, Frederick DA, Galperin A, Frankenhuis WE, Moore T (2009) Adaptive rationality: an evolutionary perspective on cognitive bias. Soc Cognit 27(5):733–763CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hawkes K (1991) Showing off tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethol Sociobiol 12(1):29–54. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(91)90011-E CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hawkes K, Bird RB (2002) Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evol Anthropol 11(2):58–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Helbing D, Yu W (2009) The outbreak of cooperation among success-driven individuals under noisy conditions. Proc Nat Acad Sci 106(10):3680–3685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci 33:61–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Herrmann B, Thöni C, Gächter S (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319:1362–1367. doi:10.1126/science.1153808 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hill K (2001) Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Hum Nat 13(1):105–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hill K, Hurtado MA (2009) Cooperative breeding in South American hunter-gatherers. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 276(1674):3863–3870Google Scholar
  43. Janssen MA, Holahan R, Lee A, Ostrom E (2010) Lab experiments for the study of social-ecological systems. Science 328(5978):613–617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Koopmans R, Rebers S (2009) Collective action in culturally similar and dissimilar groups: an experiment on parochialism, conditional cooperation, and their linkages. Evolut Hum Behav 30(3):201–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ledyard JO (1995) Public goods: a survey of experimental research. In: Kagel JH, Roth AE (eds) The handbook of experimental economics. Princeton University Press, Princeton, pp 111–194Google Scholar
  46. Leimar O, Hammerstein P (2001) Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocity. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 268:745–753. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1573 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Masclet D, Noussair C, Tucker S, Villeval M (2003) Monetary and nonmonetary punishment in the voluntary contributions mechanism. Am Econ Rev 93(1):366–380CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nikiforakis N (2008) Punishment and counter-punishment in public good games: can we really govern ourselves? J Public Econom 92(1):91–112. doi:10.1016/j.jpubeco.2007.04.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Nikiforakis N, Normann H (2008) A comparative statics analysis of punishment in public-good experiments. Exp Econ 11(4):358–369. doi:10.1007/s10683-007-9171-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ostrom E (1990) Governing the commons: the evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ostrom E (1992) Crafting institutions for self-governing irrigation systems. Institute for Contemporary Studies, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  52. Ostrom E, Gardner R, Walker J (1994) Rules, games, and common-pool resources. University of Michigan Press, Ann ArborGoogle Scholar
  53. Page T, Putterman L, Unel B (2005) Voluntary association in public goods experiments: reciprocity, mimicry efficiency. Econ J 115(506):1032–1053. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2005.01031.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Rockenbach B, Milinski M (2006) The efficient interaction of indirect reciprocity and costly punishment. Nature 444:718–723. doi:10.1038/nature05229 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Seabright P (2010) The company of strangers. A natural history of economic life, 2nd edn. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  56. Sefton M, Shupp R, Walker JM (2007) The effect of rewards and sanctions in provision of public goods. Econ Inq 45(4):671–690. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2007.00051.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Semmann D, Krambeck H, Milinski M (2003) Volunteering leads to rock–paper–scissors dynamics in a public goods game. Nature 425(6956):390–392. doi:10.1038/nature01986 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Voland E (2009) Soziobiologie—Die Evolution von Kooperation und Konkurrenz. Spektrum, Akademischer Verlag, HeidelbergGoogle Scholar
  59. West SA, Griffin AS, Gardner A (2007) Social semantics: altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. J Evolut Biol 20(2):415–432. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. West SA, El Mouden C, Gardner A (2011) Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolut Hum Behav 32(4):231–262. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wiessner P (2009) Experimental games and games of life among the Ju/’hoan Bushmen. Curr Anthropol 50(1):133–138. doi:10.1086/595622 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Yamagishi T, Kiyonari T (2000) The group as the container of generalized reciprocity. Soc Psychol Q 63(2):116–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Zelmer J (2003) Linear public goods experiments: a meta-analysis. Exp Econ 6(3):299–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Center for Philosophy and the Foundations of ScienceJustus-Liebig-University GiessenGiessenGermany

Personalised recommendations