Advertisement

Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 25, Issue 5, pp 755–780 | Cite as

Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication

  • Michael J. Owren
  • Drew Rendall
  • Michael J. Ryan
Article

Abstract

Researchers typically define animal signaling as morphology or behavior specialized for transmitting encoded information from a signaler to a perceiver. Although intuitively appealing, this conception is inherently metaphorical and leaves concepts of both information and encoding undefined. To justify relying on the information construct, theorists often appeal to Shannon and Weaver’s quantitative definition. The two approaches are, however, fundamentally at odds. The predominant definition of animal signaling is thus untenable, which has a number of undesirable consequences for both theory and practice in the field. Theoretical problems include conceptual circularity and running afoul of fundamental evolutionary principles. Problems in empirical work include that research is often grounded in abstractions such as signal honesty and semanticity, and thereby distracted from more basic and concrete factors shaping communication. A revised definition is therefore proposed, making influence rather than transmission of encoded information the central function of animal signaling. This definition is conceptually sound, empirically testable, and inclusive, yet bounded. Implications are considered in both theoretical and empirical domains.

Keywords

Animal communication Animal signaling Encoding Evolution Functional reference Influence Information Motivation Manipulation Sexual selection Shannon and Weaver 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This article is based in part on the workshop “Information and Representation in Signaling using Sound” on November 8, 2008 at Georgia State University, organized by Michael J. Owren and Walt Wilczynski, and sponsored by the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience under the STC Program of the National Science Foundation, Agreement No. IBN-9876754. Preparation of the article was partially supported by a CBN Venture Grant to Michael J. Owren, as well as a GSU RCALL Seed Grant. We thank the NIH and the NSF of the United States and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) of Canada for generous grant support over the years. Thanks to Andrea Scarantino for many helpful discussions and comments on this work, as well as to Kim Sterelny, an anonymous reviewer, and Anais Stenson.

References

  1. Bereczkei T (2001) Maternal trade-off in treating high-risk children. Evol Hum Behav 22:197–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer, SunderlandGoogle Scholar
  3. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2000) Economic models of animal communication. Anim Behav 59:259–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brooks RA (1999) Cambrian intelligence. MIT, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Bugnyar T, Kijne M, Kotrschal K (2001) Food calling in ravens: are yells referential signals? Anim Behav 61:949–958CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Capranica RR (1977) Auditory processing in anurans. Fed Proc 37:2324–2328Google Scholar
  7. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  8. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1996) Function and intention in the calls of non-human primates. Proc Br Acad 88:59–76Google Scholar
  9. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1998) Why animals don’t have language. In: Pearson GB (ed) The Tanner lectures on human values. University of Utah, Salt Lake City, pp 173–209Google Scholar
  10. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2005) Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. Linguist Rev 22:135–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Palombit R (1996) The function and mechanisms underlying baboon ‘contact’ barks. Anim Behav 52:507–518Google Scholar
  12. Cheng M-F (2008) The role of vocal self-stimulation in female responses to males: implications for state-reading. Horm Behav 53:1–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cheng M-F, Peng JP (1997) Reciprocal talk between the auditory thalamus and hypothalamus: an antidromic study. NeuroReport 8:653–658CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cummings ME (2007) Sensory trade-offs predict signal divergence in surfperch. Evolution 61:530–545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1978) Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 282–309Google Scholar
  16. de Lanarolle NC, Lang FF (1988) Functional neural pathways for vocalization in the domestic cat. In: Newman JD (ed) The physiological control of mammalian vocalization. Plenum, New York, pp 21–42Google Scholar
  17. Dong S, Clayton DF (2009) Habituation in songbirds. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92:183–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dretske FI (1981) Knowledge and the flow of information. MIT, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  19. Endler JA, Basolo AL (1998) Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends Ecol Evol 13:415–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Evans CS (1997) Referential signals. In: Owings DH, Beecher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives in ethology: vol 12 communication. Plenum, New York, pp 99–143Google Scholar
  21. Gerhardt HC, Schwartz JJ (2001) Auditory tunings and frequency preferences in anurans. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution, Washington DC, pp 73–85Google Scholar
  22. Goethals P (2003) The conduit metaphor and the analysis of meaning. In: Simon-Vandenbergen AM, Taverniers M, Ravelli L (eds) Grammatical metaphor. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 369–390Google Scholar
  23. Gould JL (1982) Ethology. WW Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  24. Green S, Marler P (1979) The analysis of animal communication. In: Marler P, Vandenbergh JG (eds) Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, vol 3: social behavior and communication. Plenum, New York, pp 73–158Google Scholar
  25. Greenewalt C (1968) Bird song: acoustics and physiology. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  26. Greenfield MD (2002) Signalers and receivers: mechanisms and evolution in arthropod communication. Oxford University, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  27. Griffiths PE, Neumann-Held EM (1999) The many faces of the gene. Bioscience 49:656–660Google Scholar
  28. Grothe B (2003) New roles for synaptic inhibition in sound localization. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:540–550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gyger M, Marler P, Pickert R (1987) Semantics of an avian alarm call system: the male domestic fowl, Gallus domesticus. Behaviour 102:15–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Haig D (1993) Genetic conflicts in human pregnancy. Q Rev Biol 68:495–532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Harrington F, Asa CS (2003) Wolf communication. In: Mech LD, Boitani L (eds) Wolves: behavior, ecology, and conservation. University of Chicago, Chicago, pp 66–103Google Scholar
  32. Hartshorne C (1973) Born to sing. Indiana University, BloomingtonGoogle Scholar
  33. Hauser MD (1996) The evolution of communication. MIT, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  34. Hertenstein MJ, Verkamp JM, Kerestes AM, Holmes RM (2006) The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: a review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 132:5–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Herzog M, Hopf S (1984) Behavioral responses to species-specific warning calls in infant squirrel monkeys reared in social isolation. Am J Primatol 7:99–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Johnstone RA (2001) Eavesdropping and animal conflict. Proc Nat Acad Sci 98:9177–9180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jürgens U (1998) Neuronal control of mammalian vocalization with special reference to the squirrel monkey. Naturwissen 85:376–388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kam Y-C, Hui WA (2002) Female–offspring communication in a Taiwanese tree frog, Chirixalus eiffingeri (Anura: Rhacophoridae). Anim Behav 64:881–886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Krebs J, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 380–402Google Scholar
  40. Krippendorff K (1993) Major metaphors of communication and some constructivist reflections on their use. Cybern Hum Knowing 1:3–25Google Scholar
  41. Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  42. LeDoux J (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Leger DW (1993) Contextual sources of information and responses to animal communication signals. Psychol Bull 113:295–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lewontin RC, Carroll SB (2001) In the beginning was the word. Science 291:1293–1295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lieberman P (2002) On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearb Phys Anthropol 45:36–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Macedonia JM (1990) What is communicated in the antipredator calls of lemurs: evidence from playback experiments with ringtailed and ruffed lemurs. Ethology 86:177–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Makagon MM, Funayama ES, Owren MJ (2008) An acoustic analysis of laughter produced by congenitally and normally hearing college students. J Acoust Soc Am 124:472–483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Manser MB, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2002) Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends Cogn Sci 6:55–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Marler P (1955) Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176:6–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Marler P, Evans CS, Hauser MD (1992) Animal signals: motivational, referential, or both? In: Papousek H, Jürgens U, Papousek M (eds) Nonverbal vocal communication: comparative and developmental approaches. Cambridge University, New York, pp 66–86Google Scholar
  51. Maynard Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal signals. Oxford University, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  52. McCowan B, Hanser SF, Doyle LR (1999) Quantitative tools for comparing animal communication systems: information theory applied to bottlenose dolphin whistles. Anim Behav 57:409–419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. McGaugh JL (2003) Memory and emotion: the making of lasting memory. Columbia University, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  54. Millikan RG (1995) A bet with peacock. In: Macdonald C, Macdonald G (eds) Philosophy of psychology: debates on psychological explanation. Oxford University, Oxford, pp 285–292Google Scholar
  55. Millikan RG (2004) The varieties of meaning. MIT, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  56. Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. O’Donnell S (2006) Polybia wasp biting interactions recruit foragers following experimental worker removals. Anim Behav 71:709–715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Otte D (1974) Effects and function in the evolution of signaling systems. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 5:385–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Owings DH (1994) How monkeys feel about the world: a review of “How monkeys see the world”. Lang Commun 14:15–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Owings DH, Morton E (1997) The role of information in communication: an assessment/management approach. In: Owings DH, Beecher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives in ethology: vol 12 communication. Plenum, New York, pp 359–390Google Scholar
  61. Owings DH, Morton E (1998) Animal vocal communication: a new approach. Cambridge University, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  62. Owren MJ, Rendall D (1997) An affect-conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocal signaling. In: Owings DH, Beecher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives in ethology: vol 12 communication. Plenum, New York, pp 299–346Google Scholar
  63. Owren MJ, Rendall D (2001) Sound on the rebound: bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocalization. Evol Anthropol 10:58–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Oyama S (2000) Ontogeny of information: developmental systems and evolution, 2nd edn. Duke University, DurhamGoogle Scholar
  65. Page R, Ryan MJ (2008) The effect of signal complexity on localization performance in bats that localize frog calls. Anim Behav 76:761–769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Penn DC, Povinelli DJ (2007) On the lack of evidence that chimpanzees possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos Trans R Soc B 362:731–744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Piccinini G, Scarantino A (in press) Computation vs. information processing: why their difference matters to cognitive science. Stud Hist Philos SciGoogle Scholar
  68. Pierce JR (1980) An introduction to information theory, symbols, signals and noise, 2nd edn. Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  69. Reddy MJ (1979) The conduit metaphor—a case of frame conflict in our language about language. In: Ortony A (ed) Metaphor and thought. Cambridge University, New York, pp 284–324Google Scholar
  70. Rendall D, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2000) Proximate factors mediating “contact” calls in adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus). J Comp Psychol 114:36–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rendall D, Owren MJ, Ryan MJ (2009) What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 78:233–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Ryan MJ (1990) Sensory systems, sexual selection, and sensory exploitation. Oxf Surv Evol Biol 7:157–195Google Scholar
  73. Ryan MJ (1998) Receiver biases, sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences. Science 281:1999–2003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Ryan MJ, Cummings M (2005) Animal signals and the overlooked costs of efficacy. Evolution 59:1160–1161Google Scholar
  75. Ryan MJ, Rand AS (1993) Sexual selection and signal evolution: the ghost of biases past. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B, Biol Sci 340:187–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Ryan MJ, Rand AS (2003) Mate recognition in túngara frogs: a review of some studies of brain, behavior, and evolution. Acta Zool Sinica 49:713–726Google Scholar
  77. Scott-Phillips TC (2008) Defining biological communication. J Evol Biol 21:387–395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Searcy WA (1992) Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. Am Zool 32:71–80Google Scholar
  79. Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of communication: reliability and deception in animal signaling systems. Princeton University, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  80. Sebeok TA (1977) How animals communicate. Indiana University, BloomingtonGoogle Scholar
  81. Seehausen O, Terai Y, Magalhaes I, Carleton K, Mrosso H, Miyagi R, van der Sluijs I, Schneider M, Maan M, Tachida H (2008) Speciation through sensory drive in cichlid fish. Nature 455:620–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Seeley TD (1995) The wisdom of the hive. Harvard University, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  83. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm calling behavior. Z Tierpsychol 54:37–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1986) Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 34:1640–1658CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2003) Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Ann Rev Psych 54:145–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Shakti L, Chandrasekhar K, Gadagkar R (2008) Signaling hunger through aggression—the regulation of foraging in a primitively eusocial wasp. Naturwissen 95:677–680CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Shannon CE, Weaver W (1949) A mathematical model of communication. University of Illinois, UrbanaGoogle Scholar
  89. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K (2005) Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Curr Biol 15:1779–1784CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Smith WJ (1977) The behavior of communicating. Harvard University, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  91. Smith WJ (1997) The behavior of communicating, after twenty years. In: Owings DH, Beecher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives in ethology: vol 12 communication. Plenum, New York, pp 7–51Google Scholar
  92. Solomon NP, Luschei ES, Liu K (1995) Fundamental frequency and tracheal pressure during three types of vocalizations elicited from anesthetized dogs. J Voice 9:403–412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Stegmann U (2005) John Maynard Smith’s notion of animal signals. Biol Philos 20:1011–1025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Stegmann U (2009) A consumer-based teleosemantics for animal signals. Philos Sci 76:864–875Google Scholar
  95. Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford University, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  97. Trivers RL (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool 14:249–264Google Scholar
  98. Voland E (1998) Evolutionary ecology of human reproduction. Annu Rev Anthropol 27:347–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. von Frisch K (1967) The dance language and orientation of bees. Harvard University, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  100. Wenner A (2002) The elusive honey bee dance “language” hypothesis. J Insect Behav 15:859–878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Wilczynski W, Chu J (2001) Acoustic communication, endocrine control, and the neurochemical systems of the brain. In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC, pp 23–35Google Scholar
  102. Wilczynski W, Lynch KS, O’Bryant EL (2005) Current research in amphibians: studies integrating endocrinology, behavior, and neurobiology. Horm Behav 48:440–450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology. Harvard University, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  104. Zahavi A (1975) Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. J Theor Biol 53:205–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle, a missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  106. Zuberbühler K (2003) Referential signalling in non-human primates: cognitive precursors and limitations for the evolution of language. Adv Study Behav 33:265–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael J. Owren
    • 1
  • Drew Rendall
    • 2
  • Michael J. Ryan
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyGeorgia State UniversityAtlantaUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of LethbridgeLethbridgeCanada
  3. 3.Section of Integrative BiologyUniversity of TexasAustinUSA

Personalised recommendations