Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 23, Issue 3, pp 317–345 | Cite as

Kinds of behaviour

Article

Abstract

Sciences able to identify appropriate analytical units for their domain, their natural kinds, have tended to be more progressive. In the biological sciences, evolutionary natural kinds are adaptations that can be identified by their common history of selection for some function. Human brains are the product of an evolutionary history of selection for component systems which produced behaviours that gave adaptive advantage to their hosts. These structures, behaviour production systems, are the natural kinds that psychology seeks. We argue these can be identified deductively by classing behaviour first according to its level of behavioural control. Early animals in our lineage used only reactive production, Vertebrates evolved motivation, and later Primates developed executive control. Behaviour can also be classified by the type of evolutionary benefit it bestows: it can deliver either immediate benefits (food, gametes), improvements in the individual’s position with respect to the world (resource access, social status), or improvements in the ability to secure future benefits (knowledge, skill). Combining history and function implies the existence of seven types of behaviour production systems in human brains responsible for reflexive, instinctual, exploratory, driven, emotional, playful and planned behaviour. Discovering scientifically valid categories of behaviour can provide a fundamental taxonomy and common language for understanding, predicting and changing behaviour, and a way of discovering the organs in the brain––its natural kinds––that are responsible for behaviour.

Keywords

Evolution Behaviour Emotion Brain Cognition Natural kinds 

References

  1. Adolphs R (2001) The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:231–239Google Scholar
  2. Allen C, Bekoff M (1997) Species of mind: the philosophy and biology of cognitive ethology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  3. Allman JM (1999) Evolving brains. Scientific American Library, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  4. Amabile TM (1996) Creativity in context. Westview Press, Boulder, CoGoogle Scholar
  5. Arnold MB (1960) Emotion and personality. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Austin JT, Vancouver JB (1996) Goal constructs in psychology: structure, process, and content. Psychol Bull 120:338–375Google Scholar
  7. Averill JR (1980) A constructionist view of emotion. In: Plutchik R, Kellerman H (eds) Emotion: theory, research, and experience. Academic Press, New York, pp 305–339Google Scholar
  8. Bandura A (1986) Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  9. Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. Am Psychol 54:462–479Google Scholar
  10. Barker RG, Wright HF (1954) Midwest and its children: the psychological ecology of an American town. Row, Peterson and Company, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  11. Barlow HB (1994) What is the computational goal of the neocortex? In: Koch C, Davis JL (eds) Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain. MIT Press, Boston, MA, pp 1–22Google Scholar
  12. Barrett LF (2006) Emotions as natural kinds? Perspectives on psychological Science 1:28–58Google Scholar
  13. Berridge KC (2004) Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol Behav 81:179–209Google Scholar
  14. Bohr N (1913) On the constitution of atoms and molecules. Philos Mag 26:1–25Google Scholar
  15. Bolker JA (2000) Modularity in development and why it matters to evo-devo. Am Zool 40:770–776Google Scholar
  16. Boyd RN (1991) Realism, anti-foundationalism and the enthusiasm for natural kinds. Philos Stud 61:127–148Google Scholar
  17. Boyd RN (1999a) Kinds, complexity and multiple realization. Philos Stud 95:67–98Google Scholar
  18. Boyd RN (1999b) Homeostasis, species, and higher taxa. In: Wilson RA (ed) Species: new interdisciplinary essays. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 141–185Google Scholar
  19. Brosnan S, Earley R, Dugatkin LA (2003) Observational learning and predator inspection in guppies. Ethology 109:823–833Google Scholar
  20. Burghardt GM (2005) The genesis of animal play. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  21. Butler AB, Hodos W (2005) Comparative vertebrate neuroanatomy: evolution and adaptation, 2nd edn. Wiley-Liss, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  22. Camerer C, Loewenstein G, Prelec D (2005) Neuroeconomics: how neuroscience can inform economics. J Econ Lit 43:9–64Google Scholar
  23. Catton C, Wilson C, Dalton R, Shotton DM (2003) SABO: a standard. Anim Behav Ontol (alpha version 0.1) vol. 2006Google Scholar
  24. Charland LC (2002) The natural kind status of emotion. Br J Philos Sci 53:511–537Google Scholar
  25. Christoff K, Gabrieli JDE (2000) The Frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology 28:168–186Google Scholar
  26. Churchland P, Sejnowski TJ (1992) The computational brain. MIT Press, Boston, MAGoogle Scholar
  27. Clark A, Karmiloff-Smith A (1993) The Cognizer’s innards: a psychological and philosophical perspective on the development of thought. Mind Lang 8:487–519Google Scholar
  28. Clore G (1994) Why emotions are felt. In: Ekman P, Davidson RJ (eds) The nature of emotion: fundamental questions. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 103–111Google Scholar
  29. Coates M, Jeffrey J, Ruta M (2002) Fins to limbs: what the fossils say. Evol Dev 4:390–401Google Scholar
  30. Cooper R (2004) Why Hacking is wrong about human kinds. Br J Philos Sci 55:73–85Google Scholar
  31. Cosmides L, Tooby J (2000) Consider the source: the evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentation: a multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Curtis V, Aunger R, Rabie T (2004) Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc: Biological Sci (Biology Letters) 277:131–133Google Scholar
  33. Curtis V, Biran A (2001) Dirt, disgust and disease. Perspect Biol Med 44:17–31Google Scholar
  34. Cziko G (1995) Without miracles: universal selection theory and the second Darwinian revolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  35. Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. G. P. Putnam’s Sons, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  36. Damasio AR (2003) Looking for spinoza. Harcourt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  37. Dawkins R (1976) Hierarchical organisation: a candidate principle for ethology. In: Bateson P, Hinde R (eds) Growing points in ethology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 7–54Google Scholar
  38. Dayan P, Abbott LF (2001) Theoretical Neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. MIT Press, Boston, MAGoogle Scholar
  39. Deci EL (1971) Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. J Pers Soc Psychol 18:105–115Google Scholar
  40. Deci EL (1975) Intrinsic motivation. Plenum Publishing Co., New YorkGoogle Scholar
  41. Deci EL, Ryan RM (1985a) Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Plenum, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  42. Deci EL, Ryan RM (1985b) Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. Delancey C (2002) Passionate engines: what emotions reveal about mind and artificial intelligence. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  44. Dennett D (1996) Kinds of minds: toward an understanding of consciousness. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. Dienes Z, Perner J (1999) A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behav Brain Sci 22:735–808Google Scholar
  46. Dolan RJ (2002) Emotion,cognition,and behavior. Science 298:1191–1194Google Scholar
  47. Druce MR, Small CJ, Bloom SR (2005) Minireview: gut peptides regulating satiety. Endocrinology 145:2660–2665Google Scholar
  48. Dupré JA (2000) Natural kinds. In: Newton-Smith WH (ed) A companion to the philosophy of science. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 311–319Google Scholar
  49. Editors (2006) The American heritage dictionary of the english language, 4th edn. Houghton Mifflin, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  50. Ekman P (1999) Basic emotion. In: Dalgleish T, Power M (eds) Handbook of cognition and emotion. Wiley, Sussex, UK, pp 45–60Google Scholar
  51. Evans D (2002) The search hypothesis of emotion. Br J Philos Sci 53:497–509Google Scholar
  52. Fagen R (1991) Animal play behavior. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  53. Fernald RD (1997) The evolution of eyes. Brain behav Evol 50:253–259Google Scholar
  54. Fincham JM, Carter CS, van Veen V, Stenger VA, Anderson JR (2002) Neural mechanisms of planning: a computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99:3346–3351Google Scholar
  55. Fishbein M, Ajzen I (1975) Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Addison–Wesley, Reading, MAGoogle Scholar
  56. Franken R (2001) Human motivation, 5th edn. Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove, CAGoogle Scholar
  57. Frederich S, Lowenstein G, O’Donoghue T (2002) Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J Econ Lit 40:351–401Google Scholar
  58. Freeman WJ (1999) How the brain makes up its mind. Weidenfeld and Nicholson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  59. Freeman WJ (2000) Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In: Lewis MD, Granic I (eds) Emotion, development, and self-organization: dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 209–235Google Scholar
  60. Fuster J (2003) Cortex and mind: unifying cognition. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  61. Gamow G (1954) Possible relation between deoxyribonucleic acid and protein structures. Nature 173:318Google Scholar
  62. Geary DC, Huffman KJ (2002) Brain and cognitive evolution: forms of modularity and functions of mind. Psychol Bull 128:667–698Google Scholar
  63. Gehring WJ, Ikeo K (1999) Pax 6: mastering eye morphogenesis and eye evolution. Trends Genet 15:371–377Google Scholar
  64. Gershenson C (2001) Artificial societies of intelligent agents. Ms Thesis thesis, Fundación Arturo Rosenblueth MéxicoGoogle Scholar
  65. Ghiselin MT (1974) A radical solution to the species problem. Syst Zool 23:536–544Google Scholar
  66. Gibbs J, Maddison SP, Rolls ET (1981) The satiety role of the small intestine in sham feeding rhesus monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol 95:1003–1015Google Scholar
  67. Godfrey-Smith P (1996) Complexity and the function of mind in nature. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  68. Golani I (1976) Homeostatic motor processes in mammalian interactions: a choreography of display. In: Bateson PPG, Klopfer PH (eds) Perspectives in ethology, vol 2. Plenum, New York, pp 69–134Google Scholar
  69. Gray JR, Braver TS, Raichle ME (2002) Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:4115–4120Google Scholar
  70. Griffiths P (1997) What emotions really are: the problem of psychological categories. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  71. Griffiths PE (1999) Squaring the circle: natural kinds with historical essences. In: Wilson RA (ed) Species: new interdisciplinary essays. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 209–228Google Scholar
  72. Griffiths PE (2004a) Is emotion a natural kind? In: Solomon RC (ed) Thinking about feeling: contemporary philosophers on emotions. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 233–249Google Scholar
  73. Griffiths PE (2004b) Emotions as natural and normative kinds. Philos Sci 71:901–911Google Scholar
  74. Hacking I (2002) Making up people. In: Hacking I (ed) Historical ontology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 99–114Google Scholar
  75. Hardcastle VG (1995) Locating consciousness. John Benjamins, Amsterdam & PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  76. Harnish R (2001) Minds, brains, computers: an historical introduction to the foundations of cognitive science. Blackwell, LondonGoogle Scholar
  77. Hartwell LH, Hopfield JJ, Leibler S, Murray AW (1999) From molecular to modular cell biology. Nature 402:C47–C52Google Scholar
  78. Haslam N (1998) Natural kinds, human kinds, and essentialism. Soc Res 65:291–314Google Scholar
  79. Haslam N (2002) Natural kinds, practical kinds and psychiatric categories: review of Zachar on biological-psychiatry. Psycoloquy 13, Biol Psychiatry 2 psycprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000193/Google Scholar
  80. Hawkins J, Blakeslee S (2004) On intelligence. Henry Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  81. Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  82. Hennig W (1966) Phylogenetic systematics. University of Illinois Press, UrbanaGoogle Scholar
  83. Hughlings Jackson J (1958) Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson, vol 1 and 2. Staples Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  84. Hull CL (1943) Principles of behavior: an introduction to behavior theory. Appleton-Century Co., New YorkGoogle Scholar
  85. Hull D (1978) A matter of individuality. Philos Sci 45:335–360Google Scholar
  86. Hutchinson GE (1944) Limnological studies in Connecticut. VII. A critical examination of the supposed relationship between phytoplankton periodicity and chemical changes in lake waters. Ecology 26:3–26Google Scholar
  87. James W (1884) What is an emotion? Mind 19:188–204Google Scholar
  88. Johnson-Laird PN (2006) How we reason. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  89. Keele S, Ivry R, Mayr U, Hazeltine E, Heuer H (2003) The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychol Rev 110:316–339Google Scholar
  90. Keller RA, Boyd RN, Wheeler QD (2003) The illogical basis of phylogenetic nomenclature. Bot Rev 69:93–110Google Scholar
  91. Kelley JL, Magurran AE (2003) Learned predator recognition and antipredator responses in fish. Fish Fish 4:216–226Google Scholar
  92. Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organisation of brain and behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  93. Kirschner MW, Gerhart JC (2006) The plausibility of life. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  94. Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F (2003) The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 302:1181–1185Google Scholar
  95. Kornblith H (2002) Knowledge and its place in nature. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  96. Kripke SA (1972) Naming and necessity. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  97. LaPorte J (2004) Natural kinds and conceptual change. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  98. Lazarus R (1991) Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  99. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Ann Rev Neurosci 23:155–184Google Scholar
  100. Lepper MR, Greene D, Nisbett RE (1973) Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: a test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol 28:129–137Google Scholar
  101. Lepper MR, Henderlong J (2000) Turning ‘play’ into ‘work’ and ‘work’ into ‘play’: 25 years of research on intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. In: Sansone C, Harackiewicz JM (eds) Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the search for optimal motivation and performance. Academic, San Diego, CA, pp 257–307Google Scholar
  102. Levenson RW (1999) The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cogn Emot 13:481–504Google Scholar
  103. Lorenz K (1950) The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns. In: Danielli JF, Brown R (eds) Physiological mechanisms in animal behaviour. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  104. Lorenz K (1965) Evolution and the modification of behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  105. Machery E (2005) Concepts are not a natural kind. Philos Sci 72:444–467Google Scholar
  106. Mallon R (2003) Social construction, social roles, and stability. In: Schmitt F (ed) Socializing metaphysics. Rowman and Littlefield, New York, pp 327–353Google Scholar
  107. Maslow AH (1943) A theory of human motivation. Psychol Rev 50:370–396Google Scholar
  108. Mayr E (1963) Animal species and evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  109. McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2007) Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci 27:5796–5804Google Scholar
  110. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2004) Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306:503–507Google Scholar
  111. Mesulam M-M (1998) From sensation to cognition. Brain 121:1013–1052Google Scholar
  112. Michaud F (2002) EMIB – Computational architecture based on emotion and motivation for intentional selection and configuration of behaviour-producing modules. Cogn Sci Q 3–4:340–361Google Scholar
  113. Miller EK, Cohen JD (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Ann Rev Neurosci 24:167–202Google Scholar
  114. Miller EK, Freedman DJ, Wallis JD (2002) The prefrontal cortex: categories, concepts, and cognition. Philos Trans: Biol Sci 357:1123–1136Google Scholar
  115. Miller GA, Galanter EH, Pribram KH (1960) Plans and the structure of behaviour. Holt Rinehart and Winston, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  116. Millikan RG (1984) Language, thought and other biological categories. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  117. Millikan RG (1989) In defense of proper functions. Philos Sci 56:288–302Google Scholar
  118. Millikan RG (1999) Historical kinds and the “special sciences”. Philos Stud 95:45–65Google Scholar
  119. Millikan RG (2000) On clear and confused ideas: an essay about substance concepts. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  120. Millikan RG (in press) Thoughts of real kinds. In: Prinz J (ed) The Oxford handbook of philosophy of psychology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  121. Mountcastle V (1957) Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat’s somatic sensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 20:408–434Google Scholar
  122. Mountcastle VB (1998) Perceptual neuroscience: the cerebral cortex. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  123. Murray HA (1938) Explorations in personality. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  124. Myers DG (2006) Psychology, 8th edn. Worth, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  125. Nesse RM (1990) Evolutionary explanations of emotion. Hum Nat 1:261–289Google Scholar
  126. Newell A (1990) Unified theories of cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  127. Northcutt RG (2002) Understanding vertebrate brain evolution. Integr Comp Biol 42:743–756Google Scholar
  128. Northcutt RG, Kaas JH (1995) The emergence and evolution of mammalian neocortex. Trends Neurosci 18:373–379Google Scholar
  129. Oatley KJ (1993) Social construction in emotion. In: Lewis M, Haviland J (eds) Handbook of emotions. Guilford Press, New York, pp 342–352Google Scholar
  130. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2005) The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 9(5):242–249Google Scholar
  131. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman M (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  132. Olds J, Milner PM (1954) Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of the septal area and other regions of the rat brain. J Comp Physiol Psychol 47:419–427Google Scholar
  133. Ortony A, Norman DA, Revelle W (2004) Affect and proto-affect in effective functioning. In: Fellous JM, Arbib MA (eds) Who needs emotions? The brain meets the robot. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 173–202Google Scholar
  134. Panksepp J (1998) Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  135. Perner J (1991) Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  136. Posner MI, DiGirolamo GJ, Fernandez-Duque D (1997) Brain mechanisms of cognitive skills. Conscious Cogn 6:267–290Google Scholar
  137. Prinz J (2004) Gut reactions: a perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  138. Proust J (2005) Rationality and metacognition in non-human animals. In: Hurley S, Nudds M (eds) Rational animals? Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 247–274Google Scholar
  139. Putnam H (1975) The meaning of ‘Meaning. In: Putnam H (ed) Mind, language, and reality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 215–271Google Scholar
  140. Pylyshyn ZW (1984) Computation and cognition: toward a foundation for cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  141. Quartz S (2001) Toward a developmental evolutionary psychology: Genes, development, and the evolution of human cognitive architecture. In: Scher S, Rauscher M (eds) Evolutionary psychology: alternative approaches. KluwerGoogle Scholar
  142. Quine WV (1969) Natural kinds. In: Rescher N (ed) Essays in honor of Carl. G. Hempel. Reidel, Dordrecht, pp 5–23Google Scholar
  143. Raby C, Alexis D, Dickinson A, Clayton N (2007) Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature 445:919–921Google Scholar
  144. Raff RA (1996) The shape of life. Chicago University Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  145. Reber A (1996) Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: an essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  146. Rieppel O (2005a) Modules, kinds, and homology. J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol 304:18–27Google Scholar
  147. Rieppel O (2005b) Monophyly, paraphyly, and natural kinds. Biol Philos 20:465–487Google Scholar
  148. Rolls ET (1999) The brain and emotion. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  149. Rolls ET (2005) Emotion explained. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  150. Roseman IJ (1984) Cognitive determinants of emotion: a structural theory. In: Shaver P (ed) Review of personality and social psychology, vol 5. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, pp 11–36Google Scholar
  151. Rosenthal DM (2000) Consciousness and metacognition. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective. Basil Blackwell, LondonGoogle Scholar
  152. Routtenberg A, Lindy J (1965) Effects of the availability of rewarding septal and hypothalamic stimulation on bar pressing for food under conditions of deprivation. J Comp Physiol Psychol 60:158–161Google Scholar
  153. Rubenstein JL, Anderson S, Shi L, Miyashita LE, Bulfone A, Hevner R (1999) Genetic control of cotrtical regionalization and connectivity. Cereb Cortex 9:524–532Google Scholar
  154. Ryan RM, Deci EL (2000) Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp Educ Psychol 25:54–67Google Scholar
  155. Savage LJ (1954) Foundations of statistics. Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  156. Schank R, Abelson R (1977) Scripts, plans goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  157. Scherer KR (1984) On the nature and function of emotion: a component process approach. In: Scherer KR, Ekman P (eds) Approaches to emotion. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 293–317Google Scholar
  158. Schleidt WM, Yakalis G, Donnelly M, McGarry J (1984) A proposal for a standard ethogram, exemplified by an ethogram of the bluebreasted quail (Coturnix chinensis). Zeitschreift für Tierpsychologie 64:193–220Google Scholar
  159. Sheffield FD, Roby TB (1950) Reward value of a non-nutritive sweet taste. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43:471–481Google Scholar
  160. Skinner BF (1938) The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  161. Sloman A (2001) Beyond shallow models of emotion. Cogn Process: Int Q Cogn Sci 2:177–198Google Scholar
  162. Sperber D (2000) Metarepresentations in evolutionary perspective. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 117–137Google Scholar
  163. Stanovich KE (2004) The Robot’s rebellion: finding meaning in the age of Darwin. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  164. Sterelny K (1990) The representational theory of mind: an introduction. Basil Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  165. Sterelny K (1998) Intentional agency and the metarepresentation hypothesis. Mind Lang 13:11–28Google Scholar
  166. Sterelny K (2003) Mind in a hostile world. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  167. Streidter GF (1998) Progress in the study of brain evolution: from speculative theories to testable hypotheses. Anat Rec 253:105–112Google Scholar
  168. Streidter GF (2005) The principles of brain evolution. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MAGoogle Scholar
  169. Suddendorf T, Busby J (2003) Mental time travel in animals? Trends Cogn Sci 7:391–396Google Scholar
  170. Sun R, Slusarz P, Terry C (2005) The interaction of the explicit and the implicit in skill learning: a dual-process approach. Psychol Rev 112:159–192Google Scholar
  171. Swanson LW (2003) Brain architecture: understanding the basic plan. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  172. Thelen E, Smith LB (1994) A dynamical systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  173. Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  174. Tooby J, Cosmides L (1990) The past explains the present: emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethol Sociobiol 11:375–424Google Scholar
  175. Tooby J, Cosmides L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 19–136Google Scholar
  176. van den Heuvel O, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ, Cath DC, van Balkom AJLM, van Hartskamp J, Barkhof F, van Dyck R (2005) Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:301–309Google Scholar
  177. von Neumann J, Morgenstern O (1944) Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  178. Wagner GP (1996) Homologues, natural kinds and the evolution of modularity. Am Zool 36:36–43Google Scholar
  179. Wagner W, Wagner GP (2003) Examining the modularity concept in evolutionary psychology: The levels of genes, mind and culture. J Cult Evol Psychol 1:135–166Google Scholar
  180. Watson JD, Crick FHC (1953) A structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid. Nature 171:737–738Google Scholar
  181. West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  182. White RW (1959) Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychol Rev 66:297–333Google Scholar
  183. Whiten A, Byrne RW (1991) The emergence of metarepresentation in human ontogeny and primate phylogeny. In: Whiten A (ed) Natural theories of mind. Basil Blackwell, LondonGoogle Scholar
  184. Wilson TD (2004) Strangers to ourselves: discovering the adaptive unconscious. Belknap Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  185. Wong R (2000) Motivation: a biobehavioural approach. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  186. Wood JN, Grafman J (2003) Human prefrontal cortex: processing and representational perspectives. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:139–146Google Scholar
  187. Zacher P (2000) Psychological concepts and biological psychiatry: a philosophical analysis. John Benjamins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  188. Zacks JM, Tversky B (2001) Event structure in perception and conception. Psychol Bull 127:3–21Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The Hygiene Centre, London School of Hygiene and Tropical MedicineLondonUK

Personalised recommendations