Advertisement

Biology & Philosophy

, Volume 23, Issue 2, pp 243–255 | Cite as

Foresight in cultural evolution

  • Alex MesoudiEmail author
Article

Abstract

Critics of Darwinian cultural evolution frequently assert that whereas biological evolution is blind and undirected, cultural change is directed or guided by people who possess foresight, thereby invalidating any Darwinian analysis of culture. Here I show this argument to be erroneous and unsupported in several respects. First, critics commonly conflate human foresight with supernatural clairvoyance, resulting in the premature rejection of Darwinian cultural evolution on false logical grounds. Second, the presence of foresight is perfectly consistent with Darwinian evolution, and is found in biology, in the form of open, teleonomic processes such as genetically-biased behavioural learning. Finally, empirical evidence from the social sciences suggests that cultural change appears far less guided and directed, and human foresight far less accurate, than is commonly assumed.

Keywords

Cultural evolution Foresight Human culture Mental time travel Niche construction Planning Teleology Teleonomy 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I wish to thank Kim Sterelny and an anonymous reviewer for several insightful comments and suggestions.

References

  1. Asch SE (1951) Effects of group pressure on the modification and distortion of judgments. In: Guetzkow H (ed) Groups, leadership and men. Carnegie, Pittsburgh, PA, pp 177–190Google Scholar
  2. Atance CM, O’Neill DK (2001) Episodic future thinking. Trends Cogn Sci 5:533–539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baldwin JM (1896) A new factor in evolution. Am Nat 30:441–451CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bandura A (1977) Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Basalla G (1988) The evolution of technology. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  6. Benton T (2000) Social causes and natural relations. In: Rose H, Rose S (eds) Alas, Poor Darwin. Harmony, New York, pp 206–224Google Scholar
  7. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  8. Bryant JM (2004) An evolutionary social science? A skeptic’s brief, theoretical and substantive. Philos Soc Sci 34:451–492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buehler R, Griffin D, Ross M (1994) Exploring the planning fallacy: why people underestimate their task completion times. J Personal Soc Psychol 67:366–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Campbell DT (1974) Evolutionary epistemology. In: Schilpp PA (ed) The philosophy of Karl Popper. Open Court, La Salle, IL, pp 413–463Google Scholar
  11. Carlson WB (2000) Invention and evolution: the case of Edison’s sketches of the telephone. In: Ziman J (ed) Technological innovation as an evolutionary process. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 137–158Google Scholar
  12. Carneiro RL (1985) Comment on Darwinian selection, symbolic variation, and the evolution of culture by D. Rindos. Curr Anthropol 26:77–78Google Scholar
  13. Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  14. Chater N (2005) Mendelian and Darwinian views of memes and cultural change. In: Hurley S, Chater N (eds) Perspectives on imitation, vol 2. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 355–362Google Scholar
  15. Darwin C (1859/1968) The origin of species. Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  16. Darwin C (1871/2003) The descent of man. Gibson Square, LondonGoogle Scholar
  17. Dasgupta S (2004) Is creativity a Darwinian process? Creat Res J 16:403–413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dawkins R (1996) The blind watchmaker. W.W. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Dennett D (1995) Darwin’s dangerous idea. Simon & Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Dennett D (2006) From typo to thinko: when evolution graduated to semantic norms. In: Levinson SC, Jaisson P (eds) Evolution and culture. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  21. Dunbar K (1995) How scientists really reason. In: Sternberg RJ, Davidson J (eds) Mechanisms of insight. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, pp 365–395Google Scholar
  22. Dunbar RIM (2003) The social brain. Ann Rev Anthropol 32:163–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Epstein JM, Axtell R (1996) Growing artificial societies. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  24. Foster PL (2004) Adaptive mutation in Escherichia coli. J Bacteriol 186:4846–4852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Garcia J, Kimeldorf DJ, Koelling RA (1955) Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122:157–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gould JL (1986) The biology of learning. Ann Rev Psychol 37:163–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Green L, Myerson J (2004) A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychol Bull 130:769–792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hallpike CR (1986) The principles of social evolution. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  29. Henrich J, Boyd R (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol Human Behav 19:215–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Henrich J, Gil White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige. Evol Human Behav 22:165–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution. Hum Nature (forthcoming)Google Scholar
  32. Jablonka E, Lamb MJ (2005) Evolution in four dimensions. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  33. James W (1880) Great men, great thoughts, and the environment. Atl Mon 46:441–459Google Scholar
  34. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1979) Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures. TIMS Stud Manage Sci 12:313–327Google Scholar
  35. Kaplan CA, Simon HA (1990) In search of insight. Cogn Psychol 22:374–419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kirby KN (1997) Bidding on the future: evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. J Exp Psychol Gen 126:54–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Köhler TA, Gumerman GJ (eds) (2000) Dynamics in human and primate societies. Oxford University Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  38. Laland KN, Sterelny K (2006) Seven reasons (not) to neglect niche construction. Evolution 60:1751–1762Google Scholar
  39. Laland KN, Kumm J, Feldman MW (1995) Gene-culture coevolutionary theory—a test-case. Curr Anthropol 36:131–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Laland KN, Odling Smee J, Feldman MW (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav Brain Sci 23:131–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Layton JC, Foster PL (2003) Error-prone DNA polymerase IV is controlled by the stress-response sigma factor, Rpos, in Escherichia coli. Mol Microbiol 50:549–561CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lipo CP, O’Brien MJ, Collard M, Shennan S (eds) (2006) Mapping our ancestors: phylogenetic approaches in anthropology and prehistory. Aldine, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. Loftus EF (1996) Eyewitness testimony. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  44. Loftus EF, Ketcham K (1994) The myth of repressed memory. St Martin’s, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. Lorenz K (1969) Innate bases of learning. In: Pribram K (ed) On the biology of learning. Harcourt, New York, pp 13–91Google Scholar
  46. Luria SE, Delbrück M (1943) Mutations of bacteria from virus sensitivity to virus resistance. Genetics 28:491–511Google Scholar
  47. Mace R, Holden CJ (2005) A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends Ecol & Evol 20:116–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mace R, Pagel MD (1994) The comparative method in anthropology. Curr Anthropol 35:549–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mayr E (1982a) Teleological and teleonomic: a new analysis. In: Plotkin H (ed) Learning, development and culture. John Wiley, Chichester, pp 17–38Google Scholar
  50. Mayr E (1982b) The growth of biological thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  51. McElreath R, Henrich J (2006) Modeling cultural evolution. In: Dunbar RIM, Barrett L (eds) Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  52. Mesoudi A (2007) Using the methods of social psychology to study cultural evolution. J Soc Evol Cult Psychol 1:35–58Google Scholar
  53. Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2004) Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the origin of species. Evolution 58:1–11Google Scholar
  54. Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Laland KN (2006a) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behav Brain Sci 29:329–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Mesoudi A, Whiten A, Dunbar RIM (2006b) A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. Br J Psychol 97:405–423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Morgan LH (1877) Ancient society. Henry Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  57. Müller M (1870) The science of language. Nature 1:256–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Newell A, Simon HA (1972) Human problem solving. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  59. O’Brien MJ, Lyman RL (2003) Cladistics and archaeology. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake CityGoogle Scholar
  60. Odling Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman M (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
  61. Paul RA (1989) Psychoanalytic anthropology. Ann Rev Anthropol 18:177–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Petroski H (1994) The evolution of useful things. Vintage, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  63. Pitt-Rivers AL (1875) On the evolution of culture. J Anthropol Inst 4:293–308Google Scholar
  64. Plotkin H (1995) Darwin machines and the nature of knowledge. Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  65. Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  66. Rosenberg SM (2001) Evolving responsively: adaptive mutation. Nat Rev Genet 2:504–515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sahlins M, Service E (1960) Evolution and culture. Ann Arbor, MichiganGoogle Scholar
  68. Schank RC, Abelson RP (1977) Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Lawrence Erlbaum, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  69. Seligman MEP (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychol Rev 77:406–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Simon HA (1982) Models of bounded rationality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  71. Simonton DK (1995) Foresight in insight? A Darwinian answer. In: Sternberg RJ (ed) The nature of insight. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 465–494Google Scholar
  72. Spencer H (1896) The principles of sociology. Appleton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  73. Sterelny K (2004) A review of evolution and learning: the Baldwin effect reconsidered, edited by B. Weber, D. Depew. Evol & Develop 6:295–300Google Scholar
  74. Sternberg RJ (1999) Darwinian creativity as a conventional religious faith. Psychol Inq 10:357–359Google Scholar
  75. Suddendorf T, Busby J (2003) Mental time travel in animals? Trends Cogn Sci 7:391–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30Google Scholar
  77. Todd PM, Gigerenzer G (2001) Précis of simple heuristics that make us smart. Behav Brain Sci 23:727–741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory. Ann Rev Psychol 53:1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185:1124–1131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tylor EB (1871) Primitive culture. John Murray, LondonGoogle Scholar
  81. Weber RJ, Perkins DN (1989) How to invent artifacts and ideas. New Ideas Psychol 7:49–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  83. Wheeler M, Ziman J, Boden MA (eds) (2002) The evolution of cultural entities. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  84. White L (1959) The evolution of culture. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  85. Ziman J (ed) (2000) Technological innovation as an evolutionary process. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Social and Developmental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations