Advertisement

Behavior Genetics

, Volume 44, Issue 3, pp 270–281 | Cite as

Estimating the Sex-Specific Effects of Genes on Facial Attractiveness and Sexual Dimorphism

  • Dorian G. Mitchem
  • Alicia M. Purkey
  • Nicholas M. Grebe
  • Gregory Carey
  • Christine E. Garver-Apgar
  • Timothy C. Bates
  • Rosalind Arden
  • John K. Hewitt
  • Sarah E. Medland
  • Nicholas G. Martin
  • Brendan P. Zietsch
  • Matthew C. Keller
Original Research

Abstract

Human facial attractiveness and facial sexual dimorphism (masculinity–femininity) are important facets of mate choice and are hypothesized to honestly advertise genetic quality. However, it is unclear whether genes influencing facial attractiveness and masculinity–femininity have similar, opposing, or independent effects across sex, and the heritability of these phenotypes is poorly characterized. To investigate these issues, we assessed facial attractiveness and facial masculinity–femininity in the largest genetically informative sample (n = 1,580 same- and opposite-sex twin pairs and siblings) to assess these questions to date. The heritability was ~0.50–0.70 for attractiveness and ~0.40–0.50 for facial masculinity–femininity, indicating that, despite ostensible selection on genes influencing these traits, substantial genetic variation persists in both. Importantly, we found evidence for intralocus sexual conflict, whereby alleles that increase masculinity in males have the same effect in females. Additionally, genetic influences on attractiveness were shared across the sexes, suggesting that attractive fathers tend to have attractive daughters and attractive mothers tend to have attractive sons.

Keywords

Facial attractiveness Masculinity–femininity Mate choice Sexual selection Intralocus sexual conflict Evolutionary genetics Twin and family studies Sex limitation 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Australian Research Council (A79600334, A79801419, and DP0212016) and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH085812 and MH63207). Thanks to Marlene Grace, Ann Eldridge, Daniel Park, and David Smyth for their work with the Australian Twin Registry; to the professional research assistants at the Center on Antisocial Drug Dependence for their work with the Longitudinal Twin Study; and to the volunteer research assistants who assigned trait ratings. Thanks to Steven W. Gangestad, Margaret J. Wright, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments on earlier drafts of this paper. And, thanks to the Australian Twin Registry and Colorado Twin Registry twins and their families for their continued participation.

Supplementary material

10519_2013_9627_MOESM1_ESM.docx (95 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 95 kb)

References

  1. Bem SL (1974) The measurement of psychological androgyny. J Consult Clin Psychol 42(2):155–162PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Boker SM, Neale MC, Maes HH, Wilde MJ, Spiegel M, Brick TR, Fox J et al (2011) OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika 76(2):306PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bonduriansky R, Chenoweth SF (2009) Intralocus sexual conflict. Trends Ecol Evol (Pers Edn) 24(5):280–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Constantinople A (1973) Masculinity–femininity: an exception to a famous dictum? Psychol Bull 80(5):389–407PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cornwell RE, Perrett DI (2008) Sexy sons and sexy daughters: the influence of parents’ facial characteristics on offspring. Anim Behav 76(6):1843–1853CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cox RM, Calsbeek R (2009) Sexually antagonistic selection, sexual dimorphism, and the resolution of intralocus sexual conflict. Am Nat 173(2):176–187PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Eaves LJ (1977) Inferring the causes of human variation. J R Stat Soc A (General) 140(3):324–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eaves LJ (2009) Putting the ‘human’ back in genetics: modeling the extended kinships of twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 12(01):1–7PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eaves LJ, Silberg JL (2003) Modulation of gene expression by genetic and environmental heterogeneity in timing of a developmental milestone. Behav Genet 33(1):1–6PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eaves LJ, Last KA, Young PA, Martin NG (1978) Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour. Heredity 41(3):249–320PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eaves LJ, Silberg JL, Foley D, Bulik C, Maes HH, Erkanli A, Worthman C (2004) Genetic and environmental influences on the relative timing of pubertal change. Twin Res 7(5):471–481PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fairbairn DJ, Roff DA (2006) The quantitative genetics of sexual dimorphism: assessing the importance of sex-linkage. Heredity 97(5):319–328PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fisher RA (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Gangestad SW, Simpson JA, Cousins AJ, Garver-Apgar CE, Christensen PN (2004) Women’s preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychol Sci 15(3):203–207PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Grammer K, Thornhill R (1994) Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness. J Comp Psychol 108(3):233–242PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Houle D (1992) Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics 130(1):195–204PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hume DK, Montgomerie R (2001) Facial attractiveness signals different aspects of “quality” in women and men. Evol Hum Behav 22(2):93–112PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jinks JL, Fulker DW (1970) Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of the human behavior. Psychol Bull 73(5):311–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Keller MC (2006) The role of mutations in human mating. In: Geher G, Miller G (eds) Mating intelligence: sex, relationships, and the mind’s reproductive system. Taylor & Francis, LondonGoogle Scholar
  20. Keller MC, Coventry WL (2005) Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Res Hum Genet 8(3):201–213PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Keller MC, Medland SE, Duncan LE (2010) Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accuracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behav Genet 40(3):377–393PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Koehler N, Simmons LW, Rhodes G (2004) How well does second-to-fourth-digit ratio in hands correlate with other indications of masculinity in males? Proc R Soc B 271(Suppl 5):S296–S298PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallam M, Smoot M (2000) Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol Bull 126(3):390–423PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Law Smith MJ, Perrett DI, Jones BC, Cornwell RE, Moorse FR, Feinberg DR et al (2006) Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc R Soc B 273:6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lee AJ, Mitchem DG, Wright MJ, Martin NG, Keller MC, Zietsch BP (2013) Genetic factors increasing male facial masculinity decrease facial attractiveness of female relatives. Psychol Sci (in press)Google Scholar
  26. Little AC, Jones BC, Burriss RP (2007) Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Horm Behav 51(5):633–639PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM (2008) Preferences for variation in masculinity in real male faces change across the menstrual cycle: women prefer more masculine faces when they are more fertile. Pers Individ Differ 45(6):478–482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mazur A, Halpern C, Udry JR (1994) Dominant looking male teenagers copulate earlier. Ethol Sociobiol 15(2):87–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McGovern RJ, Neale MC, Kendler KS (1996) The independence of physical attractiveness and symptoms of depression in a female twin population. J Psychol 130(2):209–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mealey L, Bridgstock R, Townsend GC (1999) Symmetry and perceived facial attractiveness: a monozygotic co-twin comparison. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(1):151–158PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Olson IR, Marshuetz C (2005) Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion 5(4):498–502PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI (2000) Consistency and individual differences in facial attractiveness judgments: an evolutionary perspective. Soc Res 67(1):26Google Scholar
  33. Penton-Voak IS, Jones BC, Little AC, Baker S, Tiddeman B, Burt DM, Perrett DI (2001) Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proc R Soc B 268(1476):1617–1623PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM et al (1998) Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature 394(6696):884–887PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Prokop P, Fedor P (2011) Physical attractiveness influences reproductive success of modern man. J Ethol 29:6Google Scholar
  36. Puts DA (2010) Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol Hum Behav 31(3):157–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. R Core Team (2013) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, ViennaGoogle Scholar
  38. Rhea S, Gross AA, Haberstick BC, Corley RP (2012) Colorado twin registry: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet 16:351–357PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rhen T (2000) Sex-limited mutations and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. Evolution 54(1):37–43PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rhodes G, Zebrowitz LA, Clark A, Kalick SM, Hightower A, McKay R (2001) Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health? Evol Hum Behav 22(1):31–46PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rice WR (1984) Sex chromosomes and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. Evolution 38(4):735–742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rice WR, Chippindale AK (2001) Intersexual ontogenetic conflict. J Evol Biol 14(5):685–693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Roney JR, Simmons ZL, Gray PB (2011) Changes in estradiol predict within-women shifts in attraction to facial cues of men’s testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36(5):742–749PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rowe DC, Clapp M, Wallis J (1987) Physical attractiveness and the personality resemblance of identical twins. Behav Genet 17(2):191–201PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sell A, Cosmides L, Tooby J, Sznycer D, von Rueden C, Gurven M (2009) Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proc R Soc B 276(1656):575–584. doi: 10.1098/rspb 2008.1177PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Slater A, Von der Schulenburg C, Brown E, Badenoch M, Butterworth G, Parsons S, Samuels C (1998) Newborn infants prefer attractive faces. Infant Behav Dev 21(2):345–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stearns SC, Govindaraju DR, Ewbank D, Byars SG (2012) Constraints on the coevolution of contemporary human males and females. Proc R Soc B 279(1748):4836–4844PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Swaddle JP, Reierson GW (2002) Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proc R Soc Lond B 269(1507):2285–2289. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Thornhill R, Gangestad SW (1993) Human facial beauty: averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance. Hum Nat 4(3):33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Turelli M, Barton NH (2004) Polygenic variation maintained by balancing selection: pleiotropy, sex-dependent allelic effects and G×E interactions. Genetics 166(2):1053–1079PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Weeden J, Sabini J (2005) Physical attractiveness and health in Western societies: a review. Psychol Bull 131(5):635–653PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Welling LLM, Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Conway CA, Law Smith MJ, Little AC et al (2007) Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces. Horm Behav 52(2):156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.010 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Welling LLM, Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Smith FG, Feinberg DR, Little AC, Al-Dujaili EA (2008) Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women’s faces when their testosterone levels are high. Horm Behav 54(5):703–708PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wright MJ, Martin NG (2004) Brisbane adolescent twin study: outline of study methods and research projects. Aust J Psychol 56(2):65–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dorian G. Mitchem
    • 1
    • 2
  • Alicia M. Purkey
    • 1
  • Nicholas M. Grebe
    • 3
  • Gregory Carey
    • 1
    • 2
  • Christine E. Garver-Apgar
    • 4
  • Timothy C. Bates
    • 5
  • Rosalind Arden
    • 6
  • John K. Hewitt
    • 1
    • 2
  • Sarah E. Medland
    • 7
  • Nicholas G. Martin
    • 7
  • Brendan P. Zietsch
    • 7
    • 8
  • Matthew C. Keller
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceUniversity of Colorado BoulderBoulderUSA
  2. 2.Institute for Behavioral GeneticsUniversity of Colorado BoulderBoulderUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of New MexicoAlbuquerqueUSA
  4. 4.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Colorado DenverDenverUSA
  5. 5.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  6. 6.Institute of PsychiatryKing’s CollegeLondonUK
  7. 7.QIMR Berghofer Medical Research InstituteBrisbaneAustralia
  8. 8.School of PsychologyUniversity of QueenslandBrisbaneAustralia

Personalised recommendations