Behavior Genetics

, Volume 39, Issue 4, pp 417–426 | Cite as

Genetic Covariation Between the Author Recognition Test and Reading and Verbal Abilities: What Can We Learn from the Analysis of High Performance?

  • Nicolas W. Martin
  • Narelle K. Hansell
  • Mark A. Wainwright
  • Sri N. Shekar
  • Sarah E. Medland
  • Timothy C. Bates
  • Jennifer S. Burt
  • Nicholas G. Martin
  • Margaret J. Wright
Original Research

Abstract

The Author Recognition Test (ART) measures print exposure and is a unique predictor of phonological and orthographic processes in reading. In a sample of adolescent and young adult twins and siblings (216 MZ/430 DZ pairs, 307 singletons; aged 11–29 years) ART scores were moderately heritable (67%) and correlated with reading and verbal abilities, with genes largely accounting for the covariance. We also examine whether high (and low) (i.e. 1SD above the mean) represents a quantitative extreme of the normal distribution. Heritability for high ART was of similar magnitude to the full sample, but, a specific genetic factor, independent from both low ART performance and high reading ability, accounted for 53–58% of the variance. This suggests a distinct genetic etiology for high ART ability and we speculate that the specific genetic influence is on orthographical processing, a critical factor in developing word recognition skills.

Keywords

Print exposure Verbal abilities Reading Twins Quantitative genetics 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the twins and their parents for their willingness to participate in this study. We are grateful to Ann Eldridge and Marlene Grace for the recruitment of twin pairs and data collection, David Smith and Daniel Park for computing support, as well as many other research assistants and support staff in the Genetic Epidemiology Unit at QIMR. We are thankful to the Behavior Genetics reviewers for their valuable comments on the manuscript. This work has been supported from multiple sources: Australian Research Council (A7960034, A79906588, A79801419, DP0212016, DP0343921), The Human Frontier Science Program (RG0154.1998-B), National Health and Medical Research Council (192103), and Beyond Blue (Australia). NWM is supported by a grant from the Templeton Foundation (13575).

References

  1. Aacheson DJ, Wellu JB, MacDonald MC (2008) New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behav Res Methods 40(1):278–289. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.1.278 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bates TC, Castles A, Coltheart M, Gillepsie N, Wright MJ, Martin NG (2004) Behaviour genetic analyses of reading and spelling: a component processes approach. Aust J Psychol 56:115–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bates TC, Castles A, Luciano M, Wright MJ, Coltheart M, Martin NG (2006) Genetic and environmental bases of reading and spelling: a unified genetic dual route model. Read Writ 20(1–2):51–75. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9022-1 Google Scholar
  4. Boada R, Willcutt EG, Tunick RA, Chhabildas NA, Olson RK, DeFries JC, Pennington BF (2002) A twin study of the etiology of high reading ability. Read Writ 15:683–707. doi:10.1023/A:1020965331768 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burt JS, Fury MB (2000) Spelling in adults: the role of reading skills and experience. Read Writ Interdiscip J 13:1–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Castles A, Coltheart M (1993) Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 47(2):149–180. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cherny SS, Cardon LR, Fulker DW, DeFries JC (1992) Differential heritability across levels of cognitive ability. Behav Genet 22(2):153–162. doi:10.1007/BF01066994 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Coltheart M, Rastle K, Perry C, Langdon R, Ziegler J (2001) DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol Rev 108(1):204–256. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Culbert KM, Breedlove SM, Burt SA, Klump KL (2008) Prenatal hormone exposure and risk for eating disorders: a comparison of opposite-sex and same-sex twins. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65(3):329–336. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.47 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE (1992) Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. J Educ Psychol 83(2):264–274. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.2.264 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Deary IJ, Spinath FM, Bates TC (2006) Genetics of intelligence. Eur J Hum Genet 14(6):690–700. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201588 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Detterman DK, Daniel MH (1989) Correlation of mental test with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups. Intelligence 13:349–359. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(89)80007-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Eaves LJ, Heath AC, Martin NG (1984) A note on the generalized effects of assortative mating. Behav Genet 14(4):371–376. doi:10.1007/BF01080048 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eaves LJ, Long J, Heath AC (1986) A theory of developmental change in quantitative phenotypes applied to cognitive development. Behav Genet 16(1):143–162. doi:10.1007/BF01065484 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Falconer DS (1965) The inheritance of liability to certain diseases estimated from the incidence among relatives. Ann Hum Genet 29:51–76. doi:10.1111/j.1469-1809.1965.tb00500.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Harlaar N, Dale PS, Plomin R (2007) Reading exposure: a (largely) environmental risk factor with environmentally-mediated effects on reading performance in the primary school years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48(12):1192–1199PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hirsch ED Jr (1999) The schools we need: and why we don’t have them. Anchor Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoffman ML (1977) Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychol Bull 84(4):712–722. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.4.712 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jackson DN (1984) Multidimentional aptitude battery test: manual. Research Psychologist Press, London, CanadaGoogle Scholar
  20. Kovas J, Haworth CMA, Petrill SA, Plomin R (2007a) Mathematical ability of 10-years-old boys and girls: genetic and environmental etiology of typical and low performance. J Learn Disabil 40(6):554–567. doi:10.1177/00222194070400060601 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kovas Y, Haworth CM, Dale PS, Plomin R (2007b) The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 72(3):1–144. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00453.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Loehlin JC (1996) The cholesky approach: a cautionary note. Behav Genet 26(1):65–69. doi:10.1007/BF02361160 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Loehlin JC, Martin NG (2000) Dimensions of psychological masculinity-femininity in adult twins from opposite-sex and same-sex pairs. Behav Genet 30(1):19–28. doi:10.1023/A:1002082325784 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Luciano, M. (2001) An information processing approach to the genetic study of psycometric intelligence: Links between processing speed, working memory and IQ. PhD thesis, The University of Queensland, Queensland, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
  25. Luciano M, Wright M, Smith GA, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Martin NG (2001) Genetic covariance among measures of information processing speed, working memory, and IQ. Behav Genet 31(6):581–592. doi:10.1023/A:1013397428612 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Luciano M, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Smith GA, Evans DM, Martin NG (2003) A genetic two-factor model of the covariation among a subset of multidimensional aptitude battery and wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised subtests. Intelligence 31:589–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Luciano M, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Smith GA, Martin NG (2004) A genetic investigation of the covariation among inspection time, choice reaction time, and IQ subtest scores. Behav Genet 34(1):41–50. doi:10.1023/B:BEGE.0000009475.35287.9d PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Luciano M, Lind PA, Duffy DL, Castles A, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Bates TC (2007) A haplotype spanning KIAA0319 and TTRAP is associated with normal variation in reading and spelling ability. Biol Psychiatry 62(7):811–817. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Martin NG, Eaves LJ, Kearsey MJ, Davies P (1978) The power of the classical twin study. Heredity 40(1):97–116. doi:10.1038/hdy.1978.10 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Masterson J, Hayes M (2007) Development and data for UK versions of an author and title recognition test for adults. J Res Read 30(2):212–219. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00320.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. McGregor B, Pfitzner J, Zhu G, Grace M, Eldridge A, Pearson J, Mayne C, Aitken JF, Green AC, Martin NG (1999) Genetic and environmental contributions to size, color, shape, and other characteristics of melanocytic naevi in a sample of adolescent twins. Genet Epidemiol 16(1):40–53. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1999)16:1<40::AID-GEPI4>3.0.CO;2-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nation K (2008) Learning to read words. Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester) 61(8):1121–1133. doi:10.1080/17470210802034603 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xi G, Maes HH (2003) Mx: statistical modeling. Richmond, VA: Department of PsychiatryGoogle Scholar
  34. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xi G, Maes HH (2006) Mx: statistical modeling (7th ed.). VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of PsychiatryGoogle Scholar
  35. Oakhill JV, Petrides A (2007) Sex differences in the effects of interest on boys’ and girls’ reading comprehension. Br J Psychol 98(Pt 2):223–235. doi:10.1348/000712606X117649 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Olson RK, Byrne B (2005) Genetic and environmental influences on reading and language ability and disabilities. In: Catts H, Kamhi A (eds) The connection between language and reading disabilties. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, pp 173–200Google Scholar
  37. Paulhus DL (1984) Two-component models of socially desirable responding. J Pers Soc Psychol 46:598–609CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Petrill SA, Saudino K, Cherny SS, Emde RN, Fulker DW, Hewitt JK, Plomin R (1998) Exploring the genetic and environmental etiology of high general cognitive ability in fourteen- to thirty-six-month-old twins. Child Dev 69(1):68–74PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Plomin R, Kovas Y (2005) Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychol Bull 131(4):592–617. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.592 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Plomin R, Spinath FM (2004) Intelligence: genetics, genes, and genomics. J Pers Soc Psychol 86(1):112–129. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.112 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral genetics, 5th edn. Worth Publishers, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  42. Risch N, Zhang H (1995) Extreme discordant sib pairs for mapping quantitative traits loci in human. Science 268(5217):1584–1589. doi:10.1126/science.7777857 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ronald A, Spinath FM, Plomin R (2002) The aetiology of high cognitive ability in early childhood. High Ability Stud 13(2):103–114. doi:10.1080/1359813022000048761 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rose RJ, Kaprio J, Winter T, Dick DM, Viken RJ, Pulkkinen L, Koskenvuo M (2002) Femininity and fertility in sisters with twin brothers: prenatal androgenization? Cross-sex socialization? Psychol Sci 13(3):263–267. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00448 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Silventoinen K, Posthuma D, van Beijsterveldt T, Bartels M, Boomsma DI (2006) Genetic contributions to the association between height and intelligence: evidence from dutch twin data from childhood to middle age. Genes Brain Behav 5(8):585–595. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00208.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Smith C (1974) Concordance in twins: methods and interpretation. Am J Hum Genet 26:454–466PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Stanovich KE, West RF (1989) Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Read Res Q 24(4):402–433. doi:10.2307/747605 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (1996) Using multivariates statistics, 3rd edn. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  49. Wainwright M, Wright MJ, Geffen GM, Geffen LB, Luciano M, Martin NG (2004) Genetic and environmental sources of covariance between reading tests used in neuropsychological assessment and IQ subtests. Behav Genet 34(4):365–376. doi:10.1023/B:BEGE.0000023642.34853.cb PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wechsler D (1981) Manual for the wechsler adult inteligence scale-revised (WAIS-R). The Psychological Corporation, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  51. Wright M, Martin NG (2004) Brisbane adolescent twin study: outline of study methods and research projects. Aust J Psychol 56:65–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicolas W. Martin
    • 1
    • 2
  • Narelle K. Hansell
    • 1
    • 2
  • Mark A. Wainwright
    • 1
  • Sri N. Shekar
    • 1
  • Sarah E. Medland
    • 1
    • 3
  • Timothy C. Bates
    • 4
  • Jennifer S. Burt
    • 2
  • Nicholas G. Martin
    • 1
    • 2
  • Margaret J. Wright
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Genetic Epidemiology LaboratoryQueensland Institute of Medical Research, P.O. Royal Brisbane HospitalBrisbaneAustralia
  2. 2.School of PsychologyUniversity of QueenslandBrisbaneAustralia
  3. 3.Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral GeneticsRichmondUSA
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations