Behavior Genetics

, Volume 39, Issue 4, pp 437–445 | Cite as

Generalist Genes and High Cognitive Abilities

  • Claire M. A. Haworth
  • Philip S. Dale
  • Robert Plomin
Original Research

Abstract

The concept of generalist genes operating across diverse domains of cognitive abilities is now widely accepted. Much less is known about the etiology of the high extreme of performance. Is there more specialization at the high extreme? Using a representative sample of 4,000 12-year-old twin pairs from the UK Twins Early Development Study, we investigated the genetic and environmental overlap between web-based tests of general cognitive ability, reading, mathematics and language performance for the top 15% of the distribution using DF extremes analysis. Generalist genes are just as evident at the high extremes of performance as they are for the entire distribution of abilities and for cognitive disabilities. However, a smaller proportion of the phenotypic intercorrelations appears to be explained by genetic influences for high abilities.

Keywords

Genetics High cognitive abilities Twins Generalist genes 

References

  1. Arden R, Plomin R (2007) Scant evidence for Spearman’s law of diminishing returns in middle childhood. Pers Individ Differ 42:743–753. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Davis OSP, Kovas Y, Harlaar N, Busfield P, McMillan A, Frances J et al (2008) Generalist genes and the internet generation: etiology of learning abilities by web testing at age 10. Genes Brain Behav 7:455–462. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00370.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Davis OSP, Haworth CMA, Plomin R (2009) Generalist genes for cognition: etiology of learning abilities and disabilities in early adolescence. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (in press)Google Scholar
  4. Deary IJ, Egan V, Gibson GJ, Austin EJ, Brand CR, Kellaghan T (1996) Intelligence and the differentiation hypothesis. Intelligence 23:105–132. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90008-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Deary IJ, Spinath FM, Bates TC (2006) Genetics of intelligence. Eur J Hum Genet 14:690–700. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201588 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. DeFries JC, Fulker DW (1988) Multiple regression analysis of twin data: etiology of deviant scores versus individual differences. Acta Genet Med Gemellol 37:205–216PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. GOAL plc (2002) GOAL formative assessment in literacy (key stage 3). Hodder & Stoughton, LondonGoogle Scholar
  8. Hammill DD, Brown VL, Larsen SC, Wiederholt JL (1994) Test of adolescent and adult language (TOAL-3). Pro-Ed, AustinGoogle Scholar
  9. Harlaar N, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Plomin R (2005) Reading and general cognitive ability: a multivariate analysis of 7-year-old twins. Sci Stud Read 9:197–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hartmann P (2006) Investigating Spearman’s ‘law of diminishing returns’. Aarhus University, Ph.D AarhusGoogle Scholar
  11. Hartmann P, Reuter M (2006) Spearman’s Law of diminishing returns tested with two methods. Intelligence 34:47–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Haworth CMA, Harlaar N, Kovas Y, Davis OSP, Oliver BR, Hayiou-Thomas ME et al (2007a) Internet cognitive testing of large samples needed in genetic research. Twin Res Hum Genet 10:554–563PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Haworth CMA, Meaburn EL, Harlaar N, Plomin R (2007b) Reading and generalist genes. Mind Brain Educ 1(4):173–180CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Haworth CMA, Kovas Y, Dale PS, Plomin R (2008) Science in elementary school: generalist genes and school environments. Intelligence 36:701CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Haworth CMA, Dale PS, Plomin R (2009a) Sex differences and science: the etiology of science excellence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar
  16. Haworth CMA, Kovas Y, Harlaar N, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Petrill SA, Dale PS, Plomin R (2009b) Generalist genes and learning disabilities: a multivariate genetic analysis of low performance in reading, mathematics, language and general cognitive ability in a sample of 8000 12-year-old twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (in press)Google Scholar
  17. Jensen AR (1998) The g factor: the science of mental ability. Praeger, WesportGoogle Scholar
  18. Knopik VS, Alarcón M, DeFries JC (1997) Comorbidity of mathematics and reading deficits: evidence for a genetic etiology. Behav Genet 27:447–453PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kovas Y, Plomin R (2006) Generalist genes: implications for cognitive sciences. Trends Cogn Sci 10:198–203PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kovas Y, Plomin R (2007) Learning abilities and disabilities: generalist genes, specialist environments. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:284–288CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Kovas Y, Haworth CMA, Dale PS, Plomin R (2007a) The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 72:vii–160PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Kovas Y, Haworth CMA, Harlaar N, Petrill SA, Dale PS, Plomin R (2007b) Overlap and specificity of genetic and environmental influences on mathematics and reading disability in 10-year-old twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48:914–922PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kovas Y, Haworth CMA, Petrill SA, Plomin R (2007c) Mathematical ability of 10-year-old boys and girls: genetic and environmental etiology of normal and low performance. J Learn Disabil 40:554–567PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Light JG, DeFries JC (1995) Comorbidity of reading and mathematics disabilities: genetic and environmental etiologies. J Learn Disabil 28:96–106PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Markwardt FC Jr (1997) Peabody individual achievement test-revised (normative update) manual. American Guidance Service, Circle PinesGoogle Scholar
  26. Meaburn EL, Harlaar N, Craig IW, Schalkwyk LC, Plomin R (2008) QTL association scan of early reading disability and ability using pooled DNA and 100 K SNP microarrays in a sample of 5,500 children. Mol Psychiatry 13:729–740PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Neale MC, Maes HHM (2003) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  28. nferNelson (1999) Mathematics 5–14 series. Windsor, UKGoogle Scholar
  29. Nippold MA (1998) Later language development: the school-age and adolescent years. Pro-Ed, AustinGoogle Scholar
  30. Oliver BR, Plomin R (2007) Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems from childhood through adolescence. Twin Res Hum Genet 10:96–105PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Petrill SA (2002) The case for general intelligence: a behavioral genetic perspective. In: Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL (eds) The general factor of intelligence: how general is it? Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, pp 281–298Google Scholar
  32. Plomin R (1991) Genetic risk and psychosocial disorders: links between the normal and abnormal. In: Rutter M, Casaer P (eds) Biological risk factors for psychosocial disorders. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 101–138Google Scholar
  33. Plomin R, DeFries JC (1979) Multivariate behavioral genetic analysis of twin data on scholastic abilities. Behav Genet 9:505–517PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Plomin R, Kovas Y (2005) Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychol Bull 59:2–617Google Scholar
  35. Plomin R, Spinath FM (2002) Genetics and general cognitive ability (g). Trends Cogn Sci 6:169–176PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral Genetics, 5th edn. Worth, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  37. Price TS, Freeman B, Craig IW, Petrill SA, Ebersole L, Plomin R (2000) Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Res 3:129–133PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J (1996) Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  39. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J (1998) Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices. H.K. Lewis, LondonGoogle Scholar
  40. Ronald A, Spinath F, Plomin R (2002) The aetiology of high cognitive ability in early childhood. High Abil Stud 13:103–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Saklofske DH, Yang ZM, Zhu JJ, Austin EJ (2008) Spearman’s law of diminishing returns in normative samples for the WISC-IV and WAIS-III. J Individ Differ 29:57–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA (1999) Test of word reading efficiency. Pro-Ed, AustinGoogle Scholar
  43. Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R (2002) Twins early development study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behaviour problems in childhood. Twin Res 5:444–448PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wechsler D (1992) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—third edition UK (WISC-III-UK) Manual. The Psychological Corporation, LondonGoogle Scholar
  45. Wiig EH, Secord W, Sabers D (1989) Test of language competence-expanded edition. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TXGoogle Scholar
  46. Woodcock RW, McGrew KS, Mather N (2001) Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement. Riverside Publishing, ItascaGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Claire M. A. Haworth
    • 1
  • Philip S. Dale
    • 2
  • Robert Plomin
    • 1
  1. 1.Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre P080, Institute of PsychiatryKing’s College LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.Department of Speech and Hearing SciencesUniversity of New MexicoAlbuquerqueUSA

Personalised recommendations