Behavior Genetics

, Volume 36, Issue 3, pp 355–364

Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns in Persons with Down Syndrome

Article

Abstract

Neuropsychological research has permitted defining specific cognitive profiles among individuals with mental retardation (MR) of different etiology. Namely, the cognitive profile of people with Down syndrome (DS) is often reported to be characterized by a deficit in language abilities that usually exceed impairments in visual–spatial capacities. However, recent studies have demonstrated a more complex neuropsychological profile in this population, with atypical development in the cognitive and in the linguistic domain. This paper is dedicated to reviewing literature regarding motor, linguistic and cognitive abilities in DS. Our aim is to present evidences supporting the hypothesis that individuals with these syndrome exhibit a peculiar motor development and neuropsychological profile with some abilities more preserved and others more impaired. This finding may have theoretical and practical implications. In fact, a better definition of the cognitive pattern in DS may contribute to understand the nature of MR in general and, also, it may suggests individualized rehabilitation treatment protocols.

KEY WORDS

Brain development cognition Down syndrome language memory Trisomy 21 

References

  1. Baddeley A. D., Hitch G. (1974) Working memory. In: Bower G. H. (ed) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp. 47–90Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley A. D. (1986) Working Memory. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  3. Bellugi U., Mills D., Jerningan T., Hickok G., Galaburda A. (1999) Linking cognition, brain structure, and brain function in Williams syndrome. In: Tager-Flusberg H. (ed) Neurodevelopmental Disorders. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 111–136Google Scholar
  4. Bellugi U., St. George M. (2001) Journey from Cognition to Brain to Gene. Perspectives from Williams Syndrome. MIT Press, Cambridge MAGoogle Scholar
  5. Bush A., Beail N. (2004) Risk factors for dementia in people with Down syndrome: issues in assessment and diagnosis. Am. J. Mental Retard. 109:83–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carlesimo G. A., Marotta L., Vicari S. (1997) Long-term memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down’s Syndrome. Neuropsychology 35:71–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carr J. (1970) Mental and motor development in young mongol children. J. Mental Defic. Res. 14:205–220Google Scholar
  8. Caselli M. C., Vicari S., Longobardi E., Lami L., Pizzoli C., Stella G. (1998) Gestures and words in early development of children with Down syndrome. J. Speech Hear. Res. 41:1125–1135Google Scholar
  9. Chapman R. S. (1995) Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. In: Fletcher P., MacWinney B. (eds) The Handbook of Child Language. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  10. Chapman R. S., Seung H. K., Schwartz S. E., Kay-Raining Bird E. (2000) Language production in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: the role of comprehension. J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 43:340–350Google Scholar
  11. Courtney S. M., Ungerleider L. G., Keil K., Haxby J. V. (1996) Object and visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6:39–49PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cromer R. (1987) The cognition hypothesis revisited. In: Kessel F. (ed) The Development of Language and Language Researches. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 48–65Google Scholar
  13. Davis B. (1996) Auditory disorders. In: Stratford B., Gunn P. (eds) New Approaches to Down Syndrome. Cassell, London, pp. 100–121Google Scholar
  14. Davis W. E., Kelso J. A. S. (1982) Analysis of “invariant characteristics” in motor control of Down’s syndrome and normal subjects. J. Mot. Behav. 14:194–212PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Della Sala S., Logie R. H. (2002) Neuropsychological impairments of visual and spatial working memory. In: Baddeley A. D., Wilson B., Kopelman M. (eds) Handbook of Memory Disorders. John Wiley and Sons, Chichester, pp. 271–292Google Scholar
  16. Epstein C. J. (1989) Down syndrome. In: Scriver C. R., Beaudet A. L., Sly W. S., Valle P. (eds) The Metabolic Basis of Inherited Disease. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp. 291–396Google Scholar
  17. Fabbretti D., Pizzuto E., Vicari S., Volterra V. (1997) A story description task in children with Down syndrome: lexical and morphosyntactic abilities. J. Intell. Disab. Res. 41:165–179Google Scholar
  18. Fabbro F., Alberti A., Gagliardi C., Borgatti R. (2002) Differences in native and foreign language repetition task between subjects with Williams and Down syndromes. J. Neuroling. 15: 1–10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fowler A. E. (1990) Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: evidence for a specific delay. In: Cicchetti D., Beeghley M. (eds) Children with Down Syndrome: A Developmental Perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  20. Fowler A. E. (1995) Language variability in persons with Down syndrome. In: Nadel L., Rosenthal D. (eds) Down Syndrome: Living and Learning in the Community. New York, Wiley-LissGoogle Scholar
  21. Franco F., Wishart F. (1995) The use of pointing and other gestures by young children with children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Mental Retard. 100:160–182Google Scholar
  22. Gathercole S. E., Baddeley A. D. (1993) Working Memory and Language. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hove (UK)Google Scholar
  23. Gibson D. (1978) Down Syndrome: The Psychology of Mongolism. Cambridge University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  24. Hulme, C., and Mackenzie, S. (1992). In (eds.), it Working Memory and Severe Learning Difficulties. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  25. Iverson J., Longobardi E., Caselli M. C. (2003) The relationship between gestures and words in children with Down syndrome and typically-developing children in the early stages of communicative development. Int. J. Lang. Communicat. Disorders 38:179–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jarrold C., Baddeley A. D. (1997) Short-term memory for verbal and visuo-spatial information in Down’s syndrome. Cognit. Neuropsychiat. 1:101–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jarrold C., Baddeley A. D., Hewes A. K. (1999) Genetically dissociated components of working memory: evidence from Down and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology 37:637–651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jarrold C., Baddeley A. D., Hewes A. K. (2000) Verbal short-term memory deficits in Down syndrome: a consequence of problems in rehearsal? J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 40:233–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jernigan T. L., Bellugi U., Sowell E., Doherty S., Hesselink R. (1993) Cerebral morphologic distinctions between WS and DS. Archiv. Neurol. 50:186–191Google Scholar
  30. Kanno K., Ikeda Y. (2002) Word-length effect in verbal short-term memory in individuals with Down’s syndrome. J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 46:613–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Klein B. P., Mervis C. B. (1999) Cognitive strength and weaknesses of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Develop. Neuropsychol. 16:177–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Laws G. (2002) Working memory in children and adolescents with Down syndrome: evidence from a colour memory experiment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 43:353–364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Laws G. (2004) Contributions of phonological memory, language comprehension and hearing to the expressive language of adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 45: 1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Laws G., Gunn D. (2003) Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: a five-year follow-up study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 44:1–11Google Scholar
  35. Logie R. H. (1995) Visuo-Spatial Working Memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, HoveGoogle Scholar
  36. Lydic J. S., Steele C. (1979) Assessment of the quality of sitting and gait patterns in children with Down syndrome. Phys. Therapy 59:1489–1494Google Scholar
  37. Marcell M. M. (1995) Relationships between hearing and auditory cognition in Down’s syndrome youth. Down’s Syndrome: Res. Pract. 3:75–91Google Scholar
  38. Melyn M. A., White D. T. (1973) Mental and developmental milestones of non-istitutionalized Down’s syndrome children. Pediatrics 52:542–545PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Mervis C. B., Morris C. A., Bertrand J., Robinson B. F. (1999) Williams syndrome: findings from an integrated program of research. In: Tager-Flusberg H. (ed) Neurodevelopmental Disorders. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, pp. 65–110Google Scholar
  40. Mervis C. B., Robinson B. R. (2000) Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: a comparison. Develop. Neuropsychol. 17:111–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Miller J. F. (1992) Development of speech and language in children with Down syndrome. In: Lott I. T., Mc Loy E. E. (eds) Down Syndrome. Wiley-Liss, New York, NY, pp. 39–50Google Scholar
  42. Molinari M., Leggio M. G., Solida A., Ciorra R., Misciagna S., Silveri M. et al. (1997) Cerebellum and procedural learning: evidence from focal cerebellar lesions. Brain 120:1753–1762PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nelson C. A., Monk C. S., Lin J., Carver L. J., Thomas K. M., Truwit C. L. (2000) Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. Develop. Psychol. 36:109–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nissen M. J., Bullemer P. (1987) Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognit. Psychol. 19:1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Palisano R. J., Walter S. D., Russell D. J., Rosenbaum P. L., Gémus M., Galuppi B. E. et al. (2001) Gross motor function in children with Down syndrome: creation of motor growth curves. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabilit. 82:494–500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Papagno C., Vallar G. (2001) Understanding metaphors and idioms: a single-case neuropsychological study in a person with Down syndrome. J. Internat. Neuropsychol. Soc. 7:516–528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Paterson S., Brown J. H., Gsodl, Johnson M. C., Karmiloff-Smith A. (1999) Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science 17:2355–2358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Pennington B. F., Moon J., Edgin J., Stedron J., Nadel L. (2003) The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Develop. 74:75–93PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pezzini G., Vicari S., Volterra V., Milani L., Ossella M. T. (1999) Children with Williams syndrome: is there a single neuropsychological profile? Develop. Neuropsychol. 15:141–155Google Scholar
  50. Pinter J. D., Eliez S., Schmitt J. E., Capone G. T., Reiss A. L. (2001) Neuroanatomy of Down’s syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. Am. J. Psychiat. 158:1659–1665PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rae C., Lee M. A., Dixon R. M., Blamire A. M., Thompson C. H., Styles P. et al. (1998) Brain biochemistry in Williams syndrome: evidence for a role of the cerebellum in cognition? Neurology 51:33–40PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Roizen N. J., Wolters C., Nicol T., Blondis T. A. (1993) Hearing loss in children with Down syndrome. J. Ped. 123:S9–S12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rondal J. A. (1978) Developmental sentence scoring procedure and delay-difference question in language development of Down’s syndrome children. Ment. Retard. 16:169–171Google Scholar
  54. Rondal J. A. (1993) Exceptional cases of language development in mental retardation: the relative autonomy of language as a cognitive system. In: Tager-Flusberg H. (ed) Constraints on Language Acquisition: Studies of Atypical Children. Erlbaum Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 155–174Google Scholar
  55. Rondal J. A. (1995) Exceptional Language Development in Down Syndrome. Cambridge University Press, EnglandGoogle Scholar
  56. Rondal J. A., Ghiotto M., Brédart S., Bachelet J. F. (1988) Mean length of utterance of children with Down syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 93:64–66PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Siegmuller J., Bartke S. (2004) Williams syndrome from a clinical perspective. In: Siegmuller J., Bartke S. (eds) Williams Syndrome Across Languages. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Philadelphia, USA, pp. 9–37Google Scholar
  58. Singer Harris N. G., Bellugi U., Bates E., Jones W., Rossen M. (1997) Contrasting profiles of language in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Develop. Neuropsychol. 13:345–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tomaiuolo F., Di Paola M., Caravale B., Vicari S., Petrides M., Caltagirone C. (2002) Morphology and morphometry of the corpus callosum in Williams syndrome: a magnetic resonance imaging analysis. NeuroReport 13:1–5Google Scholar
  60. Vallar G., Papagno C. (1993) Preserved vocabulary acquisition in Down’s syndrome children: the role of phonological short-term memory. Cortex 29:467–483PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Van Der Graaf F. H., De Jong B. M., Maguire R. P., Meiners L. C., Leenders K. L. (2004) Cerebral activation related to skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: striatal involvement in random-order sequence learning. Brain Res. Cognit. Brain Res. 20:120–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vicari S., Carlesimo G. A., Caltagirone C. (1995) Short-term memory in persons with intellectual disabilities and Down syndrome. J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 39:532–537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Vicari S., Caselli M. C., Tonucci F. (2000a) Early language development in Italian children with Down syndrome: asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic abilities. Neuropsychology 38:634–644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Vicari S., Bellucci S., Carlesimo G. A. (2000b) Implicit and explicit memory: a functional dissociation in persons with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia 38:240–251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Vicari S., Bellucci S., Carlesimo G. A. (2001) Procedural learning deficit in children with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology 39:665–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Vicari, S., and Carlesimo, G.A. (2002). Children with intellectual disabilities. In A. Baddeley, B. Wilson, and M. Kopelman (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., pp. 501–518Google Scholar
  67. Vicari S., Bellucci S., Carlesimo G. A. (2003) Visual and spatial working memory dissociation: evidence from a genetic syndrome. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 45:269–273PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Vicari S., Bates E., Caselli M. C., Pasqualetti P., Gagliardi C., Tonucci F. et al. (2004a) Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: an example of a dissociation between language and cognition? J. Internat. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10:862–876CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Vicari S., Marotta L., Carlesimo G. A. (2004) Verbal short-term memory in Down’s syndrome: an articulatory loop deficit? J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 48:80–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., and Carlesimo, G. A. (2005). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. in pressGoogle Scholar
  71. Wang P. P., Bellugi U. (1994) Evidence from two genetic syndromes for dissociation between verbal and visual–spatial short-term memory. J. Clin. Exper. Neuropsychol. 16:317–322Google Scholar
  72. Wisniewski K. E. (1990) Down syndrome children often have brain with maturation delay, retardation of growth, and cortical digenesis. Am. J. Med. Genet. (Suppl). 7:274–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Neurology and RehabilitationInstitute of Research and Clinical Care Bambino Gesù Children HospitalSanta Marinella, RomeItaly
  2. 2.U.O.S. Neurologia e RiabilitazioneIRCCS, Ospedale Pediatrico Bambino GesùSanta Marinella, RomeItaly

Personalised recommendations