Advertisement

Axiomathes

, Volume 18, Issue 1, pp 91–116 | Cite as

Semiotic Hypercycles Driving the Evolution of Language

  • Wolfgang WildgenEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

The evolution of human symbolic capacity must have been very rapid even in some intermediate stage (e.g. the proto-symbolic behavior of Homo erectus). Such a rapid process requires a runaway model. The type of very selective and hyperbolically growing self-organization called “hypercyle” by Eigen and Schuster could explain the rapidity and depth of the evolutionary process, whereas traditional runaway models of sexual selection seem to be rather implausible in the case of symbolic evolution. We assume two levels: at the first level the species is adapted to ecological demands and accumulates the effects of this process in the genome. At the second level a kind of social/cultural knowledge is accumulated via a set of symbolic forms, one of which is language. Bühler’s model of three basic functions of signs can also be elaborated so that its cyclic structure becomes apparent. We assume that the hypercyclic process of semiosis and functional differentiation was triggered in 2 my BP (with the Homo erectus) and got more and more speed with the species Homo sapiens and later. The consequences for the evolutionary stratification of human languages will be drawn in the last section of the paper. The basic aim of the paper is to provide a semiotic (and not just a linguistic) explanation of the origin of language which can be linked to relevant models in evolutionary biology and which exploits the possibilities contained in self-organizing systems.

Keywords

Origin of life Origin of language Language capacity Self-organization Semiotic hypercycle Cognitive hypercycle Human ecology Infinite semiosis Biosemiotics 

References

  1. Blackmore S (1999) The meme machine. Oxford UP, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Bühler K (1934/1965) Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache, 2nd edn (1965). Fischer, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  3. Cassirer E (1923/1957) The philosophy of symbolic forms, vol 1–3. Yale UP, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  4. Cumming GS, John C (2005) Change and identity in complex systems. Ecol Soc 10(1). online http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol10/iss1/art29
  5. Darwin Ch (1872) The expression of the emotions in man and animals. LondonGoogle Scholar
  6. Dawkins R (1994) Das egoistische Gen (The selfish gene). Spektrum Verlag, HeidelbergGoogle Scholar
  7. Douglas K (2001) Painted ladies. New Sci 29 October 2001Google Scholar
  8. Dunbar R (2002) Human evolutionary psychology. Palgrave-Mac Millan, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  9. Eigen M (1987) Stufen zum Leben. Die frühe Evolution im Visier der Molekularbiologie. Piper, ZürichGoogle Scholar
  10. Eigen M, Schuster P (1979) The hypercycle. A principle of natural self-organization. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  11. Fehr E, Henrich J (2003) Is strong reciprocity a maladaptation? On the evolutionary foundation of human altruism. CESIFO working papers no. 859Google Scholar
  12. Fisher RA (1930) Genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon, Oxford. Cf. the chapter on sexual selection as reprint in the internet: http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/ridley/classictexts/fisher2.pdf
  13. Fitch WT, Hauser MD, Chomsky N (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: clarifications and implications. Cognition 97:179–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Haken H (1983) Synergetik. Eine Einführung, 2nd edn. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  15. Jackendoff R, Pinker St (2005) The nature of the language faculty and its implications for the evolution of language. Cognition 97:211–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jantsch E (1984) Die Selbstorganisation des Universums. Deutscher Taschenbuch Verlag (dtv), MünchenGoogle Scholar
  17. Kirby S (2000) Syntax without natural selection: how compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In: Knight Ch, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford J (eds), The Evolutionary emergence of language. Social functions and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge UP, Cambridge, pp 303–323Google Scholar
  18. Knight Ch (1991) Blood relations: menstruation and the origin of culture. Yale UP, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  19. Lopéz-García A (2005) The grammar of genes. How the genetic code resembles the linguistic code. Lang, BernGoogle Scholar
  20. Luhmann N (1975) Einführende Bemerkungen zu einer Theorie symbolisch generalisierter Kommunikationsmedien. In: Luhmann N (ed), Soziologische Aufklärung. 2 Aufsätze zur Theorie der Gesellschaft. Westdeutscher Verlag, OpladenGoogle Scholar
  21. Martin A (1998) Organization of semantic knowledge and the origin of words in the brain. In: Jablonski N, Aiello L (eds), The origin and diversification of language. Academy of Sciences, San Francisco, pp 69–87Google Scholar
  22. Maynard Smith J, Szathmáry E (1995) The major transitions in evolution. Freeman, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  23. Niemitz C (2004) Das Geheimnis des aufrechten Gangs: unsere Evolution verlief anders. Beck, MünchenGoogle Scholar
  24. Peirce CS (1956) Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, vol 1–8. Harvard UP, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  25. Piaget J (1975) L’équilibration des structures cognitives. Problème central du développement. Presses Universitaires de France, ParisGoogle Scholar
  26. Pinker St, Bloom P (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behav and Brain Sciences 13:507–508Google Scholar
  27. Saussure F de (1916) Cours de linguistique générale. New edition: Payot, Paris 1987Google Scholar
  28. Stoneking M (1993) DNA and recent human evolution. Evol Anthropol 2:48–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Studdert-Kennedy M (2000) Evolutionary implications of the particulate principle: imitation and the dissociation of phonetic form from semantic function. In: Knight Ch, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford J (eds), The evolutionary emergence of language. Social functions and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge UP, Cambridge, pp 161–176Google Scholar
  30. Szathmáry E (2006) Neuronal evolution and the origins of language: towards a simulation platform, Contribution to the cradle of language conference, Stellenbosch (RSA), 6–10 November 2006Google Scholar
  31. Thom R (1983) Mathematical models of morphogenesis. Horwood (Wiley), New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard UP, Cambridge (Mass)Google Scholar
  33. Whitney WD (1899) Life and growth of language. An outline of linguistic science. Appleton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  34. Wilde O (1992) The picture of Dorian Gray. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Wildgen W (1982) Catastrophe theoretic semantics. An elaboration and application of René Thom’s theory. Benjamins, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  36. Wildgen W (1985) Archetypensemantik. Grundlagen für eine dynamische Semantik auf der Basis der Katastrophentheorie. Narr, TübingenGoogle Scholar
  37. Wildgen W (1987/2005) Das dynamische Paradigma in der Linguistik. Reprint 2005 with additions: http://www.fb10.uni bremen.de/homepages/wildgen/pdf/das_dynamische_paradigma.pdf
  38. Wildgen W (1994) Process, image, and meanings. A realistic model of the meanings of sentences and narrative texts. Benjamins, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  39. Wildgen W (2004a) The evolution of human languages. Scenarios, principles, and cultural dynamics. Benjamins, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  40. Wildgen W (2006) Kognitive Grammatik und neurokognitive Forschung. In: Sandkühler HJ (ed), Theorien und Begriffe der Repräsentation. Beiträge zu einem Workshop, vol 1. Schriftenreihe der Forschergruppe Repräsentation, Universität Bremen, pp 115–134Google Scholar
  41. Wildgen W (2008a) Thom’s theory of “saillance” and “prégnance” and modern evolutionary linguistics. Forthcoming in Semiosis and catastrophes. René Thom’s semiotic heritage. Brandt P, Wildgen W (eds). Lang, BernGoogle Scholar
  42. Wildgen W (2008b) Is meta-representation an effect of self-organization? Forthcoming In: Wildgen W, van Heusden B (eds), Meta-representation and (self-) consciousness: emergence of higher levels of self-organization in biological and semiotic systems. Lang, BernGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Fachbereich 10 Sprach- und LiteraturwissenschaftenUniversität BremenBremenGermany

Personalised recommendations