Advertisement

To What Extent Do Erotic Images Elicit Visuospatial versus Cognitive Attentional Processes? Consistent Support for a (Non-Spatial) Sexual Content-Induced Delay

  • Roland ImhoffEmail author
  • Paul Barker
  • Alexander F. Schmidt
Original Paper

Abstract

It is almost a cultural truism that erotic images attract our attention, presumably because paying attention to erotic stimuli provided our ancestors with mating benefits. Attention, however, can be narrowly defined as visuospatial attention (keeping such stimuli in view) or more broadly as cognitive attention (such stimuli taking up one’s thoughts). We present four independent studies aiming to test the extent to which erotic images have priority in capturing visuospatial versus cognitive attention. Whereas the former would show in quicker reactions to stimuli presented in locations where erotic images appeared previously, the latter causes delayed responding after erotic images, independent of their location). To this end, we specifically modified spatial cueing tasks to disentangle visuospatial attention capture from general sexual content-induced delay (SCID) effects—a major drawback in the previous literature. Consistently across all studies (total N = 399), we found no evidence in support of visuospatial attention capture but reliably observed an unspecific delay of responding for trials in which erotic images appeared (irrespective of cue location). This SCID is equally large for heterosexual men and women and reliably associated with their self-reported sexual excitability.

Keywords

Erotic cognition Visual attention Spatial cueing Sexual content-induced delay Sexual excitability 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The reported research and preparation of this paper were supported by a DFG Grant (IM147/3-1) awarded to Roland Imhoff. All experimental scripts, raw and aggregated data as well as results of additional analyses can be found on the Open Science Framework under https://osf.io/6uqyv.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Human and Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

10508_2019_1512_MOESM1_ESM.docx (16 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 15 kb)

References

  1. Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,134, 258–281.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aquino, J. M., & Arnell, K. M. (2007). Attention and the processing of emotional words: Dissociating effects of arousal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,14, 430–435.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion: Target misses follow attention capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion,7, 465–477.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.465.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bailey, J. M., Gaulin, S., Agyei, Y., & Gladue, B. A. (1994). Effects of gender and sexual orientation on evolutionarily relevant aspects of human mating psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,66, 1081–1093.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1081.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bancroft, J., & Janssen, E. (2000). The dual control model of male sexual response: A theoretical approach to centrally mediated erectile dysfunction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,24, 571–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartels, R. M., Lister, V. P. M., Imhoff, R., & Banse, R. (2019). Tracking mouse trajectories related to decisions about sexual interest. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48, 1387–1401.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-019-1436-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin,126, 347–374.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.347.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Both, S., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2003). Modulation of spinal reflexes by aversive and sexually appetitive stimuli. Psychophysiology,40, 174–183.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brauer, M., van Leeuwen, M., Janssen, E., Newhouse, S. K., Heiman, J. R., & Laan, E. (2012). Attentional and affective processing of sexual stimuli in women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior,41, 891–905.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9820-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Carpenter, D., Janssen, E., Graham, C., Vorst, H., & Wicherts, J. (2008). Women’s scores on the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation scales (SIS/SES): Gender similarities and differences. Journal of Sex Research,45, 36–48.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490701808076.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chivers, M. L. (2017). The specificity of women’s sexual response and its relationship with sexual orientations: A review and ten hypotheses. Archives of Sexual Behavior,46, 1161–1179.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0897-x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., & Blanchard, R. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,93, 1108–1121.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1108.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Conaglen, H. M. (2004). Sexual content induced delay: A reexamination investigating relation to sexual desire. Archives of Sexual Behavior,33, 359–367.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028889.63425.fb.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dewitte, M. (2016). Gender differences in implicit processing of sexual stimuli. European Journal of Personality,30, 107–124.  https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,18, 1030–1044.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Fox, E., Derakshan, N., & Standage, H. (2011). The assessment of human attention. In K. C. Klauer, A. Voss, & C. Stahl (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 15–47). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  17. Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130, 681–700.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fromberger, P., Jordan, K., Steinkrauss, H., Von Herder, J., Stolpmann, G., Kröner-Herwig, B., & Müller, J. L. (2013). Eye movements in pedophiles: Automatic and controlled attentional processes while viewing prepubescent stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,122(2), 587–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fromberger, P., Jordan, K., Steinkrauss, H., von Herder, J., Witzel, J., Stolpmann, G., … Müller, J. L. (2012). Diagnostic accuracy of eye movements in assessing pedophilia. Journal of Sexual Medicine,9, 1868–1882.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02754.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Geer, J. H., & Bellard, H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in unprimed lexical decisions: Gender and context effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior,25, 91–107.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02437581.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Geer, J. H., Judice, S., & Jackson, S. (1994). Reading times for erotic material: The pause to reflect. Journal of General Psychology,121, 345–352.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1994.9921208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Geer, J. H., & Melton, J. S. (1997). Sexual content-induced delay with double-entendre words. Archives of Sexual Behavior,26, 295–316.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024574915201.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Imhoff, R., Bergmann, X., Banse, R., & Schmidt, A. F. (2013). Exploring the automatic undercurrents of sexual narcissism: Individual differences in the sex-aggression link. Archives of Sexual Behavior,42, 1033–1041.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-0065-x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Imhoff, R., Lange, J., & Germar, M. (2019). Identification and location tasks rely on different mental processes: A diffusion model account of validity effects in spatial cueing paradigms with emotional stimuli. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 231–244.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1443433.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., & Gerstenberg, F. (2014). Exploring the interplay of trait self-control and ego depletion: Empirical evidence for ironic effects. European Journal of Personality,28, 413–424.  https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Nordsiek, U., Luzar, C., Young, A. W., & Banse, R. (2010). Viewing time effects revisited: Prolonged response latencies for sexually attractive targets under restricted task conditions. Archives of Sexual Behavior,39, 1275–1288.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-009-9595-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Weiß, S., Young, A. W., & Banse, R. (2012). Vicarious viewing time: Prolonged response latencies for sexually attractive targets as a function of task- or stimulus-specific processing. Archives of Sexual Behavior,41, 1389–1401.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9879-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002). The sexual inhibition (SIS) and sexual excitation (SES) scales: I. Measuring sexual inhibition and excitation proneness in men. Journal of Sex Research,39, 114–126.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490209552130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kagerer, S., Wehrum, S., Klucken, T., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2014). Sex attracts: Investigating individual differences in attentional bias to sexual stimuli. PLoS ONE,9, e107795.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0107795.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Kalmus, E., & Beech, A. R. (2005). Forensic assessments of sexual interest: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior,10, 193–217.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2003.12.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  32. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  33. Koukounas, E., & Over, R. (1999). Allocation of attentional resources during habituation and dishabituation of male sexual arousal. Archives of Sexual Behavior,28, 539–552.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018769200845.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Kambe, G. (2006). Detection of differential viewing patterns to erotic and non-erotic stimuli using eye-tracking methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior,35, 569–575.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9065-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, N. C. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution and Human Behavior,28, 28–36.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mechelsmann, D. J., Irvine, M., Banca, P., Porter, L., Mitchell, S., Mole, T. B., … Voon, V. (2014). Enhanced attentional bias towards sexually explicit cues in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PLoS ONE,9, e105476.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy,46, 656–667.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Most, S. B., Smith, S. D., Cooter, A. B., Levy, B. N., & Zald, D. H. (2007). The naked truth: Positive, arousing distractors impair rapid target perception. Cognition and Emotion,21, 964–981.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600959340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Nemetschek, R. (2013). Das Dot-Probe-Paradigma zur Erfassung sexueller Präferenzen [The Dot Probe paradigm as a measure of sexual preferences]. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Göttingen.Google Scholar
  40. Oberlader, V. A., Ettinger, U., Banse, R., & Schmidt, A. F. (2017). Development of a cued pro- and antisaccade paradigm: An indirect measure to explore automatic components of sexual interest. Archives of Sexual Behavior,46, 2377–2388.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0839-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130, 466–478.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,109, 160–174.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Prause, N., Janssen, E., & Hetrick, W. P. (2008). Attention and emotional responses to sexual stimuli and their relationship to sexual desire. Archives of Sexual Behavior,37, 934–949.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9236-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Rönspies, J., Schmidt, A. F., Melnikova, A., Krumova, R., Zolfagari, A., & Banse, R. (2015). Indirect measurement of sexual orientation: Comparison of the implicit relational assessment procedure, viewing time, and choice reaction time tasks. Archives of Sexual Behavior,44, 1483–1492.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0473-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Schmidt, A. F., Babchishin, K. M., & Lehmann, R. J. B. (2017). A meta-analysis of viewing time measures of sexual interest in children. Archives of Sexual Behavior,46, 287–300.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0806-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Schmidt, A. F., Banse, R., & Imhoff, R. (2015). Indirect measures in forensic contexts. In T. M. Ortner & F. J. R. van de Vijver (Eds.), Behavior-based assessment in psychology: Going beyond self-report in the personality, affective, motivation, and social domains (pp. 173–194). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  47. Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample sizes do correlation stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality,47, 609–612.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Seehus, M. (2015). Discrepant attentional biases toward sexual stimuli. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT.Google Scholar
  49. Snell, W. E., & Papini, D. R. (1989). The sexuality scale: An instrument to measure sexual-esteem, sexual-depression, and sexual-preoccupation. Journal of Sex Research,26, 256–263.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00224498909551510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Snowden, R. J., Curl, C., Jobbins, K., Lavington, C., & Gray, N. S. (2016). Automatic direction of spatial attention to male versus female stimuli: A comparison of heterosexual men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior,45, 843–853.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0678-y.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The Sexual Desire Inventory: Development, factor structure, and evidence of reliability. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy,22, 175–190.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00926239608414655.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Spiering, M., Everaerd, W., & Laan, E. (2004). Conscious processing of sexual information: Mechanisms of appraisal. Archives of Sexual Behavior,33, 369–380.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028890.08687.94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Vásquez-Amézquita, M., Leongómez, J. D., Seto, M. C., Bonilla, M., Rodríguez-Padilla, A., & Salvador, A. (2019). Visual attention patterns differ in gynephilic and androphilic men and women depending on age and gender of targets. Journal of Sex Research,56(1), 85–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (2011). Multiple goal management starts with attention—Goal prioritizing affects the allocation of spatial attention to goal-relevant events. Experimental Psychology,58, 55–61.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000066.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Koster, E. H., Van Damme, S., & Crombez, G. (2008). Allocation of spatial attention to emotional stimuli depends upon arousal and not valence. Emotion,8, 880–885.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013981.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Welsch, R., Schmidt, A. F., Turner, D., & Rettenberger, M. (2019). Test-retest reliability and temporal agreement of direct and indirect sexual interest measures. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  57. Wright, L., & Adams, H. (1994). Assessment of sexual preference using a choice reaction time task. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,16, 221–231.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02229209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Social and Legal PsychologyJohannes Gutenberg University MainzMainzGermany
  2. 2.Social Cognition Center CologneUniversity of CologneCologneGermany

Personalised recommendations