Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 41, Issue 1, pp 63–71 | Cite as

Considering the Role of Conditioning in Sexual Orientation

Original Paper

Abstract

The effects of learning on sexual orientation are rarely addressed in the literature. At the very least, such processes provide a means of elaborating upon orientation predispositions. Some aspects of our mates may inherently elicit a sexual response, but other attributes do so only after sexual experience with them. Animal research shows robust, direct effects of conditioning processes on partner preference with a few studies showing plasticity in preference for sex of partner. Descriptive research in humans suggests effects of experience on partner preference and, although experimental demonstrations of human sexual conditioning are neither numerous nor robust, sexual arousal is conditionable in women and men. With modern developments in learning theory (e.g., expectancy learning and evaluative conditioning), it seems appropriate to renew the investigation of contributions and limitations of conditioning processes to explaining how cues acquire erotic value and to attempt some integration between the sexual conditioning literature and research on sexual orientation or more generally sexual partner preference.

Keywords

Sexual orientation Partner preference Expectancy learning Evaluative conditioning 

References

  1. Adkins-Regan, E., & MacKillop, E. A. (2003). Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) inseminations are more likely to fertilize eggs in a context predicting mating opportunities. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270, 1685–1689. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2421.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Akins, C. K. (2000). Effects of species-specific cues and the CS–US interval on the topography of the sexually conditioned response. Learning and Motivation, 31, 211–235. doi:10.1006/lmot.2000.1050.Google Scholar
  3. Akins, C. K. (2004). The role of Pavlovian conditioning in sexual behavior: A comparative analysis of human and nonhuman animals. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17, 241–262.Google Scholar
  4. Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing implicit self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15, 498–502. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00708.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baeyens, F., Crombez, G., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1996). No evidence for modulation of evaluative flavor–flavor associations in humans. Learning and Motivation, 27, 200–241. doi:10.1006/lmot.1996.0012.Google Scholar
  6. Baeyens, F., Crombez, G., Van den Bergh, O., & Eelen, P. (1988). Once in contact always in contact: Evaluative conditioning is resistant to extinction. Advances in Behaviour Research & Therapy, 10, 179–199. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(88)90014-8.Google Scholar
  7. Baeyens, F., Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (2001). Chilled white wine, when all of a sudden the doorbell rings: Mere reference and evaluation versus expectancy and preparation in human Pavlovian learning. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (pp. 241–277). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  8. Bancroft, J. (1974). Deviant sexual behaviour: Modification and assessment. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  9. Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 347–374. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.347.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Becker, J. V., Hunter, J. A., Stein, R. M., & Kaplan, M. S. (1989). Factors associated with erection in adolescent sex offenders. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 11, 353–362. doi:10.1007/BF00961533.Google Scholar
  11. Becker, J. V., Kaplan, M. S., & Tenke, C. E. (1992). The relationship of abuse history, denial and erectile response profiles of adolescent sexual perpetrators. Behavior Therapy, 23, 87–97. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80310-7.Google Scholar
  12. Beckstead, A. L. (2012). Can we change sexual orientation? Archives of Sexual Behavior. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9922-x.
  13. Bem, D. J. (1996). Exotic becomes erotic: A developmental theory of sexual orientation. Psychological Review, 103, 320–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.320.Google Scholar
  14. Bereczkei, T. T., Gyuris, P. P., Koves, P. P., & Bernath, L. L. (2002). Homogamy, genetic similarity and imprinting: Parental influence on mate choice preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 677–690. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00182-9.Google Scholar
  15. Bereczkei, T., Gyuris, P., & Weisfeld, G. E. (2004). Sexual imprinting in human mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 1129–1134. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2672.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bickham, P. J., O’Keefe, S. L., Baker, E., Berhie, G., Kommor, M. J., & Harper-Dorton, K. V. (2007). Correlates of early overt and covert sexual behaviors in heterosexual women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 724–740. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9220-1.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Both, S., Laan, E., Spiering, M., Nilsson, T., Oomens, S., & Everaerd, W. (2008a). Appetitive and aversive classical conditioning of female sexual response. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 1386–1401. doi:10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.00815.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Both, S., Spiering, M., Laan, E., Belcome, S., Van den Heuvel, B., & Everaerd, W. (2008b). Unconscious classical conditioning of sexual arousal: Evidence for the conditioning of female genital arousal to subliminally presented sexual stimuli. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 100–109. doi:10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.00815.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer, D. S. Palermo, W. B. Weimer, & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (pp. 1–42). Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. Burton, D. L. (2003). Male adolescents: Sexual victimization and subsequent sexual abuse. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 20, 277–296. doi:10.1023/A:1024556909087.Google Scholar
  21. Carew, T. J., Hawkins, R. D., & Kandel, E. R. (1983). Differential classical conditioning of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. Science, 219, 397–400. doi:10.1126/science.6681571.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Cibrian-Llanderal I. L., Rosas-Aguilar, V., Triana-Del Rio, R., Pérez-Estudillo, C. A., Manzo, J., Garcia, L. I., & Coria-Avila, G. A. (submitted). Can homosexual partner preference be learned in adulthood? Findings from an animal model. The Journal of Sexual Medicine.Google Scholar
  23. Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Extensive training, partial reinforcement, and temporal gaps do not affect S–S learning in second-order autoshaping. Animal Learning & Behavior, 13, 165–170.Google Scholar
  24. Coria-Avila, G. A., Gavrila, A. M., Boulard, B., Charron, N., Stanley, G., & Pfaus, J. G. (2008a). Neurochemical basis of conditioned partner preference in the female rat: II. Disruption by flupenthixol. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 396–406. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.396.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Coria-Avila, G. A., Jones, S. L., Solomon, C. E., Gavrila, A. M., Jordan, G. J., & Pfaus, J. G. (2006). Conditioned partner preference in female rats for strain of male. Physiology & Behavior, 88, 529–537. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.001.Google Scholar
  26. Coria-Avila, G. A., Ouimet, A. J., Pacheco, P., Manzo, J., & Pfaus, J. G. (2005). Olfactory conditioned partner preference in the female rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 716–725. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.716.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Coria-Avila, G. A., Solomon, C. E., Vargas, E., Lemme, I., Ryan, R., Ménard, S., et al. (2008b). Neurochemical basis of conditioned partner preference in the female rat: I. Disruption by naloxone. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 385–395. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.385.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Coria-Avila, G. A., Triana Del Rio, R., Montero-Dominguez, F., Hernandez, M. E., & Manzo, J. (2009, Oct). Olfactory conditioned partner preference between male rats is induced by the D2-like specific agonist quinpirole. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Chicago.Google Scholar
  29. Cusato, B., & Domjan, M. (1998). Special efficacy of sexual conditioned stimuli that include species typical cues: Tests with a conditioned stimuli preexposure design. Learning and Motivation, 29, 152–167. doi:10.1006/lmot.1997.0988.Google Scholar
  30. Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50. doi:10.1126/science.1188595.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Davey, H. M. (1994). Flow cytometry of microorganisms. Unpublished thesis, Institute of Biological Sciences, University of Wales.Google Scholar
  32. De Houwer, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative conditioning. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 230–241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. De Houwer, J., Baeyens, F., & Field, A. P. (2005). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: Some current controversies and possible ways forward. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 161–174. doi:10.1080/02699930441000265.Google Scholar
  34. De Houwer, J., Hendrickx, H., & Baeyens, F. (1997). Evaluative learning with ‘subliminally’ presented stimuli. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 87–107. doi:10.1006/ccog.1996.0281.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.127.6.853.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among young sexual-minority women over a 2-year period. Developmental Psychology, 36, 241–250. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Diamond, L. M. (2006). The evolution of plasticity in female–female desire. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 18, 245–274. doi:10.1300/J056v18n04_01.Google Scholar
  38. Diamond, L. M. (2009). Sexual fluidity: Understanding women’s love and desire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Diamond, L. M., & Fagundes, C. P. (2008). Developmental perspectives on links between attachment and affect regulation over the lifespan. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 83–134. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(08)00003-7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Dickinson, A., & Brown, K. J. (2007). Flavor-evaluative conditioning is unaffected by contingency knowledge during training with color-flavor compounds. Learning & Behavior, 35, 36–42.Google Scholar
  41. Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I don’t know why: Enhancing implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345–355. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.345.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Domjan, M. (2005). Pavlovian conditioning: A functional perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 179–206. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Domjan, M., & Akins, C. K. (2011). Applications of Pavlovian conditioning to sexual behavior and reproduction. In T. R. Schachtman & S. Reilly (Eds.), Associative learning and conditioning theory: Human and non-human applications (pp. 507–531). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Domjan, M., Blesbois, E., & Williams, J. (1998). The adaptive significance of sexual conditioning: Pavlovian control of sperm release. Psychological Science, 9, 411–415. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00077.Google Scholar
  45. Domjan, M., & Hall, S. (1986). Determinants of social proximity in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica): Male behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 59–67. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.100.1.59.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Domjan, M., & Holloway, K. S. (1998). Sexual learning. In G. Greenberg & M. M. Haraway (Eds.), Comparative psychology: A handbook (pp. 602–613). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  47. Fillion, T. J., & Blass, E. M. (1986). Infantile experience with suckling odors determines adult sexual behavior in male rats. Science, 231, 729–731. doi:10.1126/science.3945807.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Freund, K., & Blanchard, R. (1993). Erotic target location errors in male gender dysphorics, paedophiles, and fetishists. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 558–563. doi:10.1192/bjp.162.4.558.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Galef, B. R. (2008). Social influences on the mate choices of male and female Japanese quail. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Review, 3, 1–12. doi:10.3819/ccbr.2008.30001.Google Scholar
  50. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: Conceptual, empirical, and metatheoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 745–750. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.745.Google Scholar
  51. Graham, J. M., & Desjardins, C. (1980). Classical conditioning: Induction of luteinizing hormone and testosterone secretion in anticipation of sexual activity. Science, 210, 1039–1041. doi:10.1126/science.7434016.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Gyuris, P., Járai, R., & Bereczkei, T. (2010). The effect of childhood experiences on mate choice in personality traits: Homogamy and sexual imprinting. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 467–472. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.021.Google Scholar
  53. Hardwick, S. A., & Lipp, O. V. (2000). Modulation of affective learning: An occasion for evaluative conditioning? Learning and Motivation, 31, 251–271. doi:10.1006/lmot.2000.1051.Google Scholar
  54. Hermans, D., Crombez, G., Vansteenwegen, D., Baeyens, F., & Eelen, P. (2002a). Expectancy-learning and evaluative learning in human classical conditioning: Differential effects of extinction. In S. P. Shohov & S. P. Shohov (Eds.), Advances in psychology research (pp. 17–40). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.Google Scholar
  55. Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Eelen, P. (2002b). Expectancy learning and evaluative learning in human classical conditioning: Affective priming as an indirect and unobtrusive measure of conditioned stimulus valence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 217–234. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00006-7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Hilliard, S., Nguyen, M., & Domjan, M. (1997). One-trial appetitive conditioning in the sexual behavior system. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 237–241.Google Scholar
  57. Hoffmann, H. (2007, Aug). Classical conditioning of sexual arousal in men and women with an olfactory CS and vibrotactile US: Evidence for signal and affective learning. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Vancouver, BC.Google Scholar
  58. Hoffmann, H. (2011). Hot and bothered: Classical conditioning of sexual incentives in humans. In T. R. Schachtman & S. Reilly (Eds.), Associative learning and conditioning theory: Human and non-human applications (pp. 532–550). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Hoffmann, H., & Janssen, E. (2006, July). Classical conditioning of sexual arousal to an olfactory cue in women and men: Who learns and what is learned? Paper presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  60. Hoffmann, H., Janssen, E., & Turner, S. (2004). Classical conditioning of sexual arousal in women and men: Effects of varying awareness and biological relevance of the conditioned stimulus. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 43–53. doi:10.1023/B:ASEB.0000007461.59019.d3.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Hoffmann, H., Peterson, K., & Garner, H. (2009, Aug). “Field” conditioning of sexual arousal in humans. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, San Juan, PR.Google Scholar
  62. Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421. doi:10.1037/a0018916.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Hollis, K. L. (1997). Contemporary research on Pavlovian conditioning: A “new” functional analysis. American Psychologist, 52, 956–965.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Hollis, K. L., Pharr, V. L., Dumas, M. J., Britton, G. B., & Field, J. (1997). Classical conditioning provides paternity advantage for territorial male blue gouramis (Trichogaster trichopterus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 219–225. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.111.3.219.Google Scholar
  65. Hollis, K. L., ten Cate, C., & Bateson, P. (1991). Stimulus representation: A subprocess of imprinting and conditioning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 307–317. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.105.4.307.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Hunter, J. A., Goodwin, D. W., & Becker, J. V. (1994). The relationship between phallometrically measured deviant sexual arousal and clinical characteristics in juvenile sexual offenders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 533–538. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90142-2.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Immelmann, K., Pröve, R., Lassek, R., & Bischof, H. (1991). Influence of adult courtship experience on the development of sexual preferences in zebra finch males. Animal Behaviour, 42, 83–89. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80608-6.Google Scholar
  68. Ismail, N., Gelez, H., Lachapelle, I., & Pfaus, J. G. (2009). Pacing conditions contribute to the conditioned ejaculatory preference for a familiar female in the male rat. Physiology & Behavior, 96, 201–208. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.09.013.Google Scholar
  69. Jespersen, A. F., Lalumière, M. L., & Seto, M. C. (2009). Sexual abuse history among adult sex offenders and non-sex offenders: A meta-analysis. Child Abuse and Neglect, 33, 179–192. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.07.004..PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Junginger, J. (1997). Fetishism: Assessment and treatment. In D. R. Laws & W. T. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 92–110). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  71. Kantorowitz, D. (1978). An experimental investigation of preorgasmic reconditioning and postorgasmic deconditioning. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 23–34. doi:10.1901/jaba.1978.11-23.Google Scholar
  72. Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774–788. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.774.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Kendrick, K. M., Hinton, M. R., Atkins, K., Haupt, M. A., & Skinner, J. D. (1998). Mothers determine sexual preference. Nature, 395, 229–230. doi:10.1038/26129.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445–1452. doi:10.1126/science.3629249.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Kippin, T. E., & Pfaus, J. G. (2001). The nature of the conditioned response mediating olfactory conditioned ejaculatory preference in the male rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 122, 11–24. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00162-0.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Kirsch, I., Lynn, S., Vigorito, M., & Miller, R. R. (2004). The role of cognition in classical and operant conditioning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 369–392. doi:10.1002/jclp.10251.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Klucken, T., Tabbert, K., Schweckendiek, J., Merz, C., Kagerer, S., Vaitl, D., et al. (2009). Contingency learning in human fear conditioning involves the ventral striatum. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 3636–3644. doi:10.1002/hbm.20791.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Köksal, F., & Domjan, M. (1998). Observational conditioning of sexual behavior in the domesticated quail. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26, 427–432.Google Scholar
  79. Köksal, F., Domjan, M., & Weisman, G. (1994). Blocking of the sexual conditioning of differentially effective conditioned stimulus objects. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 103–111. doi:10.3758/bf03199962.Google Scholar
  80. Krause, M. A., Cusato, B., & Domjan, M. (2003). Extinction of conditioned sexual responses in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica): Role of species typical cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 76–86. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Lalumière, M. L., & Quinsey, V. L. (1998). Pavlovian conditioning of sexual interests in human males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 27, 241–252. doi:10.1023/A:1018686817316.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Lipp, O. V., & Purkis, H. M. (2005). No support for dual process accounts of human affective learning in simple Pavlovian conditioning. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 269–282. doi:10.1080/02699930441000319.Google Scholar
  83. Lipp, O. V., & Purkis, H. M. (2006). The effects of assessment type on verbal ratings of conditional stimulus valence and contingency judgments: Implications for the extinction of evaluative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 431–440. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.32.4.431.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Evolution and individual differences in the perception of attractiveness: How cyclic hormonal changes and self-perceived attractiveness influence female preferences for male faces. In G. Rhodes & L. A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives (pp. 59–90). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
  85. Livingston, R. W., & Drwecki, B. B. (2007). Why are some individuals not racially biased? Susceptibility to affective conditioning predicts non-prejudice towards Blacks. Psychological Science, 18, 816–823. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01985.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Mahometa, M. J., & Domjan, M. (2005). Classical conditioning increases reproductive success in Japanese quail, Coturnix japonica. Animal Behaviour, 69, 983–989. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.023.Google Scholar
  87. Manrique, T., & Gutierrez, G. (2006). Effects of early social experiences on mating preferences of Coturnix japonica. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 9, 57–73.Google Scholar
  88. Matthews, R. N., Domjan, M., Ramsey, M., & Crews, D. (2007). Learning effects on sperm competition and reproductive fitness. Psychological Science, 18, 758–762. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01974.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. McGuire, R. J., Carlisle, J. M., & Young, B. G. (1965). Sexual deviations as conditioned behaviour: A hypothesis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2, 185–190. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(64)90014-2.Google Scholar
  90. Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 23–38. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.1.23.Google Scholar
  91. Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–198. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09000855.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Money, J. (1984). Paraphilias: Phenomenology and classification. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 38, 164–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Money, J. (1986). Lovemaps: Clinical concepts of sexual/erotic health and pathology, paraphilia, and gender transposition in childhood, adolescence, and maturity. New York: Irvington Publishers.Google Scholar
  94. Nash, S., & Domjan, M. (1991). Learning to discriminate the sex of conspecifics in male Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica): Tests of “biological constraints”. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 342–353. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.17.3.342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Neuman, R., Forster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch & K. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 371–391). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  96. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 12, 413–417. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00376.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Paredes, R. G., & Alonso, A. (1997). Sexual behavior regulated (paced) by the female induces conditioned place preference. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 123–128. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.123.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I., Little, A. C., Tiddeman, B., Burt, D. M., Schmidt, N., et al. (2002). Facial attractiveness judgments reflect learning of parental age characteristics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269, 873–880. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.1971.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., & Centeno, S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: A review. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 291–321. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1686.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Plaud, J. J., & Martini, J. R. (1999). The respondent conditioning of male sexual arousal. Behavior Modification, 23, 254–268. doi:10.1177/0145445599232004.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Rahman, Q. (2005). The neurodevelopment of human sexual orientation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 1057–1066.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Rantala, M. J., Pölkki, M., & Rantala, L. M. (2010). Preference for human male body hair changes across the menstrual cycle and menopause. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 419–423. doi:10.1093/beheco/arp206.Google Scholar
  104. Razran, G. (1971). Mind in evolution. An East–West synthesis of learned behavior and cognition. New York: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  105. Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43, 151–160. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.3.151.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1998). The experimental analysis of human sexual arousal: Some recent developments. Behavior Analyst, 21, 37–52.Google Scholar
  107. Rose, M., Haider, H., & Büchel, C. (2005). Unconscious detection of implicit expectancies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 918–927. doi:10.1162/0898929054021193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Rozin, P., & Zellner, D. (1985). The role of Pavlovian conditioning in the acquisition of food likes and dislikes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 443, 189–202. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb27073.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Santtila, P., Högbacka, A., Jern, P., Johansson, A., Varjonen, M., Witting, K., et al. (2009). Testing Miller’s theory of alleles preventing androgenization as an evolutionary explanation for the genetic predisposition for male homosexuality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 58–65. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.08.004.Google Scholar
  110. Seto, M. C., Abramowitz, C. S., & Barbaree, H. E. (2008). Paraphilias. In L. Hunsley & E. J. Mash (Eds.), A guide to assessments that work (pp. 488–512). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  111. Seto, M. C., & Lalumière, M. L. (2010). What is so special about male adolescent sexual offending? A review and test of explanations using meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 526–575. doi:10.1037/a0019700.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Snowdon, C. T., Tannebaum, P. L., Schultz-Darken, N. J., Ziegler, T. E., & Ferris, C. F. (2011). Conditioned sexual arousal in a nonhuman primate. Hormones and Behavior, 59, 696–701. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.10.009.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Spence, R., & Smith, C. (2007). The role of early learning in determining shoaling preferences based on visual cues in the Zebrafish, Danio rerio. Ethology, 113, 62–67. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01295.x.Google Scholar
  114. Storms, M. D. (1981). A theory of erotic orientation development. Psychological Review, 88, 340–353. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Sullivan, R. (2001). Unique characteristics of neonatal classical conditioning: The role of the amygdala and locus coeruleus. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 36, 293–307.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. ten Cate, C., Verzijden, M. N., & Etman, E. (2006). Sexual imprinting can induce sexual preferences for exaggerated parental traits. Current Biology, 16, 1128–1132. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.068.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Timberlake, W. (2001). Motivational modes in behavior systems. In R. R. Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 155–209). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  118. Wallen, K. (2001). Sex and context: Hormones and primate sexual motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 339–357. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1696.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. Walther, E., & Nagengast, B. (2006). Evaluative conditioning and the awareness issue: Assessing contingency awareness with the four-picture recognition test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 454–459. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.32.4.454.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Calhan, C. (1994). Homosexual foot fetishism. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 611–626. doi:10.1007/BF01541815.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. White, D. J. (2004). Influences of social learning on mate-choice decisions. Learning & Behavior, 32, 105–113.Google Scholar
  122. Wiszewska, A., Pawlowski, B., & Boothroyd, L. G. (2007). Father-daughter relationship as a moderator of sexual imprinting: A facialmetric study. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 248–252. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.02.006.Google Scholar
  123. Woodson, J. C. (2002). Including “learned sexuality” in the organization of sexual behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 69–80. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00039-2.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Woodson, J. C., Balleine, B. W., & Gorski, R. A. (2002). Sexual experience interacts with steroid exposure to shape the partner preferences of rats. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 148–157. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1816.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  125. Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 464–476. doi:10.1038/nrn1919.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1048–1054. doi:10.1038/nn1327.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyKnox CollegeGalesburgUSA

Personalised recommendations