Advertisement

Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 39, Issue 5, pp 1073–1079 | Cite as

Subliminal Sexual Stimuli Facilitate Genital Response in Women

  • Jorge PonsetiEmail author
  • Hartmut A. G. Bosinski
Original Paper

Abstract

Visual sexual stimuli (VSS) are believed to undergo an automatic process of stimulus appraisal and (genital or subjective) response generation. Consistent with this belief, studies have found that subliminal VSS can facilitate responses to subsequent sexual stimulus presentations. We tested whether subliminal sexual stimuli facilitated a genital response in women and, furthermore, whether this genital response could be modulated by both opposite-sex stimuli and same-sex stimuli (i.e., whether the genital response to subliminal stimuli is category-specific or nonspecific). Twenty heterosexual women underwent vaginal photoplethysmography while being subliminally (20 ms) exposed to same-sex, opposite-sex, and nonsexual slides in a priming experiment. Vaginal pulse amplitude was increased when target stimuli were preceded by both opposite-sex and same-sex priming stimuli relative to nonsexual priming stimuli. This finding suggests that subliminal VSS were subjected to automatic stimulus processing, thereby facilitating nonspecific genital response preparation. Results are discussed in terms of implicit and explicit memory access and the evolutionary benefit of female nonspecific genital response.

Keywords

Memory Sexual arousal Subliminal Backward masking Vaginal photoplethysmography 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Subliminal picture presentation was enabled by a software tool particularly developed for the requirements of this study by Roger Dow from Contact Precision Instruments. We wish to thank Roger for his very sufficient technical and IT support.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  2. Baars, B. J. (1989). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University press.Google Scholar
  3. Bancroft, J. (1989). Human sexuality and its problems (2nd ed.). Edinburgh, Scotland: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
  4. Barlow, D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction: The role of anxiety and cognitive interference. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 140–148.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 347–374.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Both, S., Spiering, M., Laan, E., Belcome, S., van den Heuvel, B., & Everaerd, W. (2008). Unconscious classical conditioning of sexual arousal: Evidence for the conditioning of female genital arousal to subliminally presented sexual stimuli. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 100–109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, A., & Hamm, A. (1993). Affective picture processing. In N. Birbaumer & A. Öhman (Eds.), The structure of emotion (pp. 48–65). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  8. Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984). Visual masking: An integrative approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  9. Buss, D. M. (2000). Desires in human mating. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907, 39–49.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chivers, M. L. (2005). A brief review and discussion of sex differences in the specificity of sexual arousal. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 20, 377–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2005). A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biological Psychology, 70, 115–120.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chivers, M. L., Seto, M. C., & Blanchard, R. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in sexual response to sexual activities versus gender of actors in sexual films. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1108–1121.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Deacon, D., Uhm, T. J., Ritter, W., Hewitt, S., & Dynowska, A. (1999). The lifetime of automatic semantic priming effects may exceed two-seconds. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 465–472.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 204–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Franke, G. (1996). Die Symptom-Checkliste von Derogatis (Deutsche Version). Göttingen, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
  17. Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Does subliminal exposure to sexual stimuli have the same effects on men and women? Journal of Sex Research, 44, 111–121.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Hoffmann, H., Janssen, E., & Turner, S. L. (2004). Classical conditioning of sexual arousal in women and men: Effects of varying awareness and biological relevance of the conditioned stimulus. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 43–53.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Janssen, E., & Everaerd, W. (1993). Determinants of male sexual arousal. Annual Review of Sex Research, 4, 211–245.Google Scholar
  21. Janssen, E., Everaerd, W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli: Toward an information processing model of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 8–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kirk, K. M., Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Measurement models for sexual orientation in a community twin sample. Behavior Genetics, 30, 345–356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kuban, M., Barbaree, H. E., & Blanchard, R. (1999). A comparison of volume and circumference phallometry: Response magnitude and method agreement. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28, 345–359.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Laan, E., Everaerd, W., van Bellen, G., & Hanewald, G. (1994). Women’s sexual and emotional responses to male- and female-produced erotica. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 153–169.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
  26. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  27. Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2002). The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 9527–9532.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Öhman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23, 123–145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231–240.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Ponseti, J., Bosinski, H. A., Wolff, S., Peller, M., Jansen, O., Mehdorn, H. M., et al. (2006). A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. Neuroimage, 33, 825–833.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Sergent, C., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1391–1400.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Sintchak, G., & Geer, J. H. (1975). A vaginal plethysmograph system. Psychophysiology, 12, 113–115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Spiering, M., & Everaerd, W. (2007). The sexual unconscious. In E. Janssen (Ed.), The psychophysiology of sex (pp. 166–184). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Spiering, M., Everaerd, W., & Janssen, E. (2003). Priming the sexual system: Implicit versus explicit activation. Journal of Sex Research, 40, 134–145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Spiering, M., Everaerd, W., Karsdorp, P., Both, S., & Brauer, M. (2006). Nonconscious processing of sexual information: A generalization to women. Journal of Sex Research, 43, 268–281.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Steinman, D. L., Wincze, J. P., Sakheim, D. K., Barlow, D. H., & Mavissakalian, M. (1981). A comparison of male and female patterns of sexual arousal. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 10, 529–547.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Weinstein, M. C., & Fineberg, H. V. (1980). Clinical decision analysis. Philadelphia: Saunders Company.Google Scholar
  38. Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Section of Sexual Medicine, Medical SchoolChristian-Albrechts-University of KielKielGermany

Personalised recommendations