Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Sex Differences in Personality Traits and Gender-Related Occupational Preferences across 53 Nations: Testing Evolutionary and Social-Environmental Theories

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using data from over 200,000 participants from 53 nations, I examined the cross-cultural consistency of sex differences for four traits: extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, and male-versus-female-typical occupational preferences. Across nations, men and women differed significantly on all four traits (mean ds = −.15, −.56, −.41, and 1.40, respectively, with negative values indicating women scoring higher). The strongest evidence for sex differences in SDs was for extraversion (women more variable) and for agreeableness (men more variable). United Nations indices of gender equality and economic development were associated with larger sex differences in agreeableness, but not with sex differences in other traits. Gender equality and economic development were negatively associated with mean national levels of neuroticism, suggesting that economic stress was associated with higher neuroticism. Regression analyses explored the power of sex, gender equality, and their interaction to predict men’s and women’s 106 national trait means for each of the four traits. Only sex predicted means for all four traits, and sex predicted trait means much more strongly than did gender equality or the interaction between sex and gender equality. These results suggest that biological factors may contribute to sex differences in personality and that culture plays a negligible to small role in moderating sex differences in personality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In analyses of data from 57 cultures, McCrae (2002) reported that correlations between men’s and women’s SDs for NEO facets ranged from .42 to .88, across cultures, with a median value of .68 (see also McCrae et al., 2005b). This suggests that not only do trait means vary in consistent ways across cultures, but trait variances do as well. In explaining these results, McCrae (2002) wrote, “It is not yet clear whether the consistency is due to the culture or to the language. In the Filipino subsamples, facet SDs were, on average, 14% larger when the NEO-PI-R was administered in Filipino than when it was administered in English” (p. 114).

  2. To probe the impact of other dimensions of cultural variation, I also analyzed national scores on Hofstede’s (1991) four cultural dimensions: power distance (the acceptance of unequal distributions of power in organizations and institutions), individualism versus collectivism, masculinity versus femininity (instrumental versus communal values in work-related settings), and uncertainty avoidance (the degree to which members of various cultures are uncomfortable with uncertainty and ambiguity). Hofstede (1991) presents scores on these dimensions for 50 nations, 40 of which overlapped with the nations assessed in the current study (see also Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005; Peabody, 1999).

    When nations’ scores on Hofstede’s four cultural dimensions were correlated with the UN gender and economic development scores used in the current study, correlations were high for two of Hofstede’s dimensions: power distance and individualism/collectivism. Specifically, the Hofstede power dimension correlated −.58 with UN gender development, −.68 with UN gender empowerment, −.36 with life expectancy, and −.70 with per capita income (all ps < .001 expect for the third, which was <.05), and individualism/collectivism dimension correlated with the same indices .62, .73, .36, and .75 (p values the same as before). These generally strong correlations imply that nations high on gender equality and economic development tended also to be individualistic nations that had relatively low inequality in their organizational power distributions—findings that are consistent with the results of other studies (see Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005; McCauley, Ottati, & Lee, 1999). The other two Hofstede dimensions—masculinity-femininity and uncertainty avoidance—were not significantly related to UN or economic indices, nor were they related much to sex differences in personality.

    Because of the strong overlap between the Hofstede power distance and individualism/collectivism dimensions and the other indices assessed in the current study, I report here only results for the UN gender equality and economic development indices. The fact that two of the Hofstede dimensions correlated strongly with the UN gender equality and economic development measures lends some ambiguity to the proper interpretation of the cultural dimension that is tapped by the UN measures. On the other hand, the strong intercorrelation of these four quite different measures suggests that the dimension of cultural variation that is being assessed in common by all four measures is a fundamental one.

References

  • Archer, J., & Mehdikhani, M. (2003). Variability among males in sexually selected attributes. Review of General Psychology, 7, 219–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2002). Cultural suppression of female sexuality. Review of General Psychology, 6, 166–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benet-Martínez, V., & John, O. P. (1998). Los cinco grandes across cultures and ethnic groups: Multitrait multimethod analysis of the Big Five in Spanish and English. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 729–750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2005). Sex differences in the design features of socially contingent mating adaptations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 278–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality across cultures: Robust and surprising results. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50, 145–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2005). Universal sex differences across patriarchal cultures ≠ evolved psychological dispositions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 281–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guimond, S., Branscombe, N. R., Brunot, S., Buunk, A. P., Chatard, A., Desert, M., et al. (2007). Culture, gender, and the self: Variations and impact of social comparison processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1118–1134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J. H., & Downs, D. (1980). Mothers, fathers and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behavior in young children. Child Development, 51, 1217–1247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. (1998). Gender-related individual difference and the structure of vocational interests: The importance of the “People-Things” dimension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 996–1009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2001). On deconstructing and reconstructing masculinity-femininity. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 168–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2007). The preferred traits of mates in a cross-national study of heterosexual and homosexual men and women: An examination of biological and cultural influences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 198–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (2008). Sex differences and sexual orientation differences in personality: Findings from the BBC Internet survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 173–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A. (in press). Sex differences in sex drive, sociosexuality, and height across 53 nations: Testing evolutionary and social structural theories. Archives of Sexual Behavior.

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181–1209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, C., Ottati, V., & Lee, Y. (1999). National differences in economic growth: The role of personality and culture. In Y. Lee, C. R. McCauley, & J. G. Draguns (Eds.), Personality and person perception across cultures (pp. 85–100). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R. (2002). NEO-PI-R data from 36 cultures: Further intercultural comparisons. In R. R. McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The five-factor models of personality across cultures (pp. 105–128). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005a). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 547–561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005b). Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 407–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moffit, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Sex differences in antisocial behavior. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Peabody, D. (1999). National characteristics: Dimensions for comparison. In Y. Lee, C. R. McCauley, & J. G. Draguns (Eds.), Personality and person perception across cultures (pp. 65–84). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prediger, D. J. (1982). Dimensions underlying Holland’s hexagon: Missing link between interests and occupations? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 21, 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimers, S. (2007). The BBC sex differences study: General methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 147–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 3): Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 993–1016). New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 247–311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. (1981). On the self-perpetuating nature of social stereotypes. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 183–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swann, W. B. (1999). Resilient identities: Self-relationships and the construction of social reality. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tracey, T. J. G., & Rounds, J. (1993). Evaluating Holland’s and Gati’s vocational interest models: A structural meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes: A multi-nation study. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of men and women: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699–727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

I am grateful to BBC TV Science for commissioning this research, and to the BBC Science and Nature website for programming and hosting the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard A. Lippa.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lippa, R.A. Sex Differences in Personality Traits and Gender-Related Occupational Preferences across 53 Nations: Testing Evolutionary and Social-Environmental Theories. Arch Sex Behav 39, 619–636 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9380-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9380-7

Keywords

Navigation