Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 37, Issue 4, pp 598–604 | Cite as

Differences in Gait Across the Menstrual Cycle and Their Attractiveness to Men

  • Meghan P. Provost
  • Vernon L. Quinsey
  • Nikolaus F. Troje
Original Paper

Abstract

We investigated variations in gait between women at high and at low conception probability, and how men rated those variations. Women participated in a motion capture study where we recorded the kinematics of their walking patterns. Women who were not using hormonal contraception (n = 19) repeated the study during the late follicular stage and the luteal stage of their menstrual cycle. Using a discriminant function analysis, we found significant differences in walking behavior between naturally cycling women at their follicular and luteal phases, with 71% of the walks classified correctly. However, there was no difference between walks of women in their follicular stage and women using hormonal birth control (n = 23). We compared structural and kinematic characteristics of the women’s walking patterns that appeared to be characteristic of women in the specific conception risk groups, but found no significant differences. In a second study, 35 men rated the walks of women not using hormonal contraception as slightly more attractive during the luteal stage of the cycle compared to the late follicular stage. Thus, for women not using hormonal birth control, it would appear that some information regarding female fertility appears to be encoded in gait.

Keywords

Unadvertised ovulation Motion capture Gait Biological motion Attractiveness 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank A. Olmos, T. Otto, D. Saunders, and C. Westhoff for their assistance with the motion capture technology and computer programming. We would like to thank Dr. S. Fergus for his assistance with HLM. We would also like to thank the Editor and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. We would also like to acknowledge the support of a SSHRC scholarship to MPB, and a grants from CFI and OIT to NFT.

References

  1. Badcock, C. R. (1991). Evolution and individual behavior: An introduction to human sociobiology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  2. Barclay, C. D., Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1978). Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 145–152.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of ones movements without seeing ones body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22.Google Scholar
  4. Cutting, J. E. (1978). A program to generate synthetic walkers as dynamic point-light displays. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 10, 91–94.Google Scholar
  5. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cures. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.Google Scholar
  6. Davis, R. B., Ounpuu, S., Tyburski, D., & Gage, J. R. (1991). A gait analysis data collection and reduction technique. Human Movement Science, 10, 575–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dittrich, W. H. (1993). Action categories and the perception of biological motion. Perception, 22, 15–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Doty, R., Ford, M., Preti, G., & Huggins, G. (1975). Changes in the intensity and pleasantness of human vaginal odors during the menstrual cycle. Science, l90, 1316–1318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Freeman, A. L. J., & Wong, H. Y. (1995). The evolution of self-concealed ovulation in humans. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 531–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gangestad S. W., Thornhill R., & Garver C. E. (2002). Changes in women’s sexual interests and their partners’ mate retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 975–982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Grammer, K., Filova, V., & Fieder, M. (1997). The communication paradox and possible solutions: Towards a radical empiricism. In A. Schmitt, K. Atzwanger, K. Grammer, & K. Schaefer (Eds.), New aspects of human ethology (pp. 91–120). London: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Guida, M., Barbato, M., Bruno, P., Lauro, G., & Lampariello, C. (1993). Salivary ferning and the menstrual cycle in women. Clinical Experiments in Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 20, 48–54.Google Scholar
  13. Guida, M., Tommaselli, G. A., Palomba, S., Pellicano, M., Moccia, G., Di Carlo, C., et al. (1999). Efficacy of methods for determining ovulation in a natural family planning program. Fertility and Sterility, 79, 900–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hewett, T. E. (2000). Neuromuscular and hormonal factors associated with knee injuries in female athletes: Strategies for intervention. Sports Medicine, 29, 313–327.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.Google Scholar
  16. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  17. Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–580.Google Scholar
  18. MacDonald, R. R. (1969). Cyclic changes in cervical mucus. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology of the British Commonwealth, 76, 1090–1099.Google Scholar
  19. Mather G., & Murdoch L. (1994). Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 258, 273–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Morris, N. M., & Udry J. R. (1970). Variations in pedometer activity during the menstrual cycle. Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 35, 199–201.Google Scholar
  21. Raudenbush, S., Bryk, A., & Congdon, R. (2005). HLM (Version 6.02) [Computer software]. Lincolnwood, IL: SSI Scientific Software International.Google Scholar
  22. Roberts, S. C., Havlicek, J., Flegr, J., Hruskova, M., Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., et al. (2004). Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, S270–S272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Salvatore, C. A. (1961). Mechanism of cervical mucus crystallization. Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 18, 598–606.Google Scholar
  24. Schröder, I. (1993). Concealed ovulation and clandestine copulation: A female contribution to human evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 381–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Strassmann, B. I. (1981). Sexual selection, paternal care, and concealed ovulation in humans. Ethology and Sociobiology, 2, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Troje, N. F. (2002a). Decomposing biological motion: A framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. Journal of Vision, 2, 371–387.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Troje, N. F. (2002b). The little difference: Fourier based gender classification from biological motion. In R. P. Würtz & M. Lappe (Eds.), Dynamic perception (pp. 115–120). Berlin: Aka Press.Google Scholar
  28. Troje, N. F., Westhoff, C., Lavrov, M. (2005) Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 667–675.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Meghan P. Provost
    • 1
  • Vernon L. Quinsey
    • 1
  • Nikolaus F. Troje
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyQueen’s UniversityKingstonCanada

Personalised recommendations