Archives of Sexual Behavior

, Volume 35, Issue 6, pp 699–709

Associations Among Gender-Linked Toy Preferences, Spatial Ability, and Digit Ratio: Evidence from Eye-Tracking Analysis

Article

Eye-tracking technology was used to monitor eye-movements in 64 adults (age range, 18–22 years) during simultaneous presentation of “masculine” and “feminine” toys. Women and men who showed more visual fixations on male-typical toys compared to female-typical toys had significantly better targeting ability and smaller (i.e., more masculine) digit ratios, a putative marker of prenatal androgen levels. In contrast, individuals with visual preferences for female-typical or male-typical toys did not differ in mental rotations ability and in their retrospective reports of childhood gender-linked activities. The finding that targeting ability and digit ratios varied according to visual preferences for gender-linked toys suggests that prenatal androgens promote enduring preferences for male-typical objects and indicate further that some gender-linked traits vary according to the direction of a visual preference for gender-linked toys. Visual preferences derived from eye-tracking, therefore, may be a useful supplement to current measures of psychosexual differentiation in hormone-behavior research, particularly because eye-movements are not dependent on verbal abilities or subjective evaluations of behavior.

KEY WORDS:

sex differences toy preferences spatial ability androgens eye-tracking 

REFERENCES

  1. Alexander, G. M. (2003). An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: Pink, blue and the brain. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 7–14.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (1994). Gender labels and play styles: Their relative contribution to children's selection of playmates. Child Development, 65, 869–879.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in responses to children's toys in a non-human primate (cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Benenson, J. F., Duggan, V., & Markovits, H. (2004). Sex differences in infants’ attraction to group versus individual stimuli. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 173–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Benenson, J. F., Liroff, E. R., Pascal, S. J., & Cioppa, G. D. (1997). Propulsion: A behavioural expression of masculinity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 37–50.Google Scholar
  7. Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3, 203–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Breedlove, S. M., Cooke, B. M., & Jordan, C. L. (1999). The orthodox view of brain sexual differentiation. Brain, Behavior & Evolution, 54, 8–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brown, W. M., Hines, M., Fane, B. A., & Breedlove, S. M. (2002). Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 380–386.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Buck, J. J., Williams, R. M., Hughes, I. A., & Acerini, C. G. (2003). In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio: Comparisons between healthy control and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Human Reproduction, 18, 976–979.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social-cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Caldera, Y. M., Culp, A. M., O’Brien, M., Truglio, R. T., Alvarez, M., & Huston, A. C. (1999). Children's play preferences, construction play with blocks, and visual-spatial skills: Are they related? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 855–872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Heywood, C. (2000). Infants’ visual preference for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 479–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cohen-Bendahan, C. C. C., van de Beek, C., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2005). Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: Methods and findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 353–384.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  16. Collaer, M. L., & Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioral sex differences: A role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 55–107.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine- and feminine-activity preferences scales for preschoolers: Correlates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development, 48, 1411–1416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Coolican, J., & Peters, M. (2003). Sexual dimorphism in the 2d/4d ratio and its relation to mental rotation performance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 179–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Csatho, A., Osvath, A., Karadi, K., Bicsak, E., Manning, J., & Kallai, J. (2003). Spatial navigation related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 239–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. H., Kappes, M. E., Borger, D., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Stegner, H., et al. (1990). Congenital adrenal hyperplasia: II, Gender-related behavior and attitudes is female salt-wasting and simple-virilizing patients. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15, 421–434.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Ehrhardt, A. A., & Baker, S. W. (1974). Fetal androgens, human central nervous system differentiation, and behavior sex differences. In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. L. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp. 33–52). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  23. Ehrhardt, A. A., & Money, J. (1967). Progestin-induced hermaphroditism: IQ and psychosexual identity in a study of ten girls. Journal of Sex Research, 3, 83–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eisenberg, N., Murray, E., & Hite, T. (1982). Children's reasoning regarding sex-typed toy choices. Child Development, 53, 81–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fagot, B. I. (1978). The influence of sex of child on parental reactions to toddler children. Child Development, 49, 459–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M., & O’Boyle, C. (1992). Gender labeling, gender stereotyping and parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 28, 225–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fagot, B. I., & Littman, I. (1976). Relation of preschool sex-typing to intellectual performance in elementary school. Psychological Reports, 39, 699–704.Google Scholar
  28. Gaulin, S. J. C., & Fitzgerald, R. W. (1989). Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability. Animal Behavior, 37, 322–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1993). The Pre-School Activities Inventory: A standardized assessment of gender role in children. Psychological Assessment, 5, 131–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Goy, R. W. (1980). Early hormonal influences on the development of sexual and sex-related behavior. In F. O. Schmitt (Ed.), The neurosciences: Second study program (pp. 196–206). New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Grimshaw, G. M., Sitarenios, G., & Finegan, J. K. (1995). Mental rotation at 7 years: Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition, 29, 85–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Hall, J. A. Y., & Kimura, D. (1995). Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimorphic motor tasks. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 395–407.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2004). Sexually differentiated toy preferences in rhesus monkeys. Paper presented at the Society for Behavioral Neuroendocrinology Annual Meeting, Lisbon, Portugal.Google Scholar
  34. Hines, M., Brook, C., & Conway, G. S. (2004). Androgen and psychosexual development: Core gender identity, sexual orientation, and recalled childhood gender role behavior in women and men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Journal of Sex Research, 41, 75–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hines, M., Fane, B. A., Pasterski, V. L., Mathews, G. A., Conway, G. S., & Brooke, C. (2002a). Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: Targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 1010–1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K. J., Golding, J., & ALSAP. (2002b). Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: A longitudinal, population study. Child Development, 73, 1678–1687.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Hines, M., & Kaufman, F. R. (1994). Androgen and the development of human sex-typical behavior: Rough-and-tumble play and sex of preferred playmates in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (cah). Child Development, 65, 1042–1053.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 23, 315–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kerns, K. A., & Berenbaum, S. A. (1991). Sex differences in spatial ability in children. Behavior Genetics, 21, 383–396.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1990). Mothers, fathers and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children. Child Development, 51, 1237–1247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1974). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Liss, M. B. (1981). Patterns of toy play: An analysis of sex differences. Sex Roles, 7, 1143–1150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Loftus, G. R., & Mackworth, N. H. (1978). Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 565–572.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Manning, J. T., Bundred, P. E., Newton, D. J., & Flanagan, B. F. (2003). The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 399–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Manning, J. T., Scott, D., Wilson, J., & Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: A predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Human Reproduction, 13, 3000–3004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1987). The roles of cognition in sex role acquisition. In D. B. Carter (Ed.), Current conceptions of sex roles and sex typing: Theory and research (pp. 123–137). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  50. Martin, C. L., & Little, J. K. (1990). The relation of gender understanding to children's sex-typed preferences and gender stereotypes. Child Development, 61, 1427–1439.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Carlson, A. D., Obeid, J. S., & New, M. I. (2004). Prenatal androgenization affects gender-related behavior but not identity in 5–12-year-old girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 97–104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Morgan, B. L. (1998). A three-generational study of tomboy behavior. Sex Roles, 39, 787–800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. O’Brien, M., & Huston, A. C. (1985a). Activity level and sex stereotyped toy choice in toddler boys and girls. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 146, 527–534.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. O’Brien, M., & Huston, A. C. (1985b). Development of sex-typed play behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 21, 866–871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Otken, A., Kalyoncu, M., & Yaris, N. (2002). The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Human Development, 70, 47–54.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Panksepp, J., Siviy, S., & Normansell, L. (1984). The psychobiology of play: Theoretical and methodological perspectives. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 8, 465–492.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76, 264–278.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Peters, M., Laing, B., Latham, D., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn vandenberg & kuse mental rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life. Sex Roles, 22, 359–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Putz, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sporter, R. J., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate? Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 182–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Richardson, D., & Spivey, M. (2000). Representation, space, and hollywood squares: Looking at things that aren't there anymore. Cognition, 76, 269–295.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Robert, M., & Heroux, G. (2004). Visuo-spatial play experience: Forerunner of visuo-spatial achievement in preadolescent and adolescent boys and girls? Infant and Child Development, 13, 49–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children's play preferences and patterns of cognitive performance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 315–316.Google Scholar
  64. Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., Colburne, K. A., Sen, M. G., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2001). Gender stereotyping in infancy: Visual preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the second year of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 7–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sherman, J. (1967). Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of intellectual functioning. Psychological Review, 74, 290–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sprafkin, C., Serbin, L. A., Denier, C., & Connor, J. M. (1983). Sex-differentiated play: Cognitive consequences and early interventions. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and children's play (pp. 167–192). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  67. Thomas, J. R., & French, K. E. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260–282.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Tracy, D. M. (1987). Toys, spatial ability, and science and mathematics achievement: Are they related? Sex Roles, 17, 115–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical pathways. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  70. van Anders, S. M., & Hampson, E. (2005). Testing the prenatal androgen hypothesis: Measuring digit ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial abilities in adults. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 92–98.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotation, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualisation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 47, 599–604.Google Scholar
  72. Voyer, D., Nolan, C., & Voyer, S. (2000). The relation between experience and spatial performance in men and women. Sex Roles, 43, 891–915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Voyer, D., & Saunders, K. A. (2004). Gender differences on the mental rotations test: A factor analysis. Acta Psychologica, 117, 79–94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A metal-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Watson, N. V. (2001). Sex differences in throwing: Monkeys having a fling. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 98–99.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Wilson, F. A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Viewing preferences of rhesus monkeys related to memory for complex pictures, colours and faces. Behavioural Brain Research, 60, 79–89.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Zucker, K. J. (2005). Measurement of psychosexual differentiation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 375–388.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyTexas A&M UniversityCollege StationUSA

Personalised recommendations