Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback

, Volume 31, Issue 2, pp 115–128

The Effect of Emotional Distance on Psychophysiologic Concordance and Perceived Empathy Between Patient and Interviewer

Article

This preliminary study investigated the effect of emotional distance on psychophysiologic concordance and perceived empathy in a clinical population. Participants included 20 adult outpatients from a mental health clinic that underwent a brief semi-structured interview with a trained psychiatrist in either an emotionally neutral or an emotionally distant condition. Simultaneous skin conductance (SC) levels of the patient and interviewer were recorded and used to calculate a measure of psychophysiologic concordance. Interviewer gaze was rated by an independent observer and used as a proxy indicator of emotional distance. Observer ratings of interviewer gaze, SC concordance, and patient ratings of perceived interviewer empathy were significantly lower in the emotionally distant condition compared with the emotionally neutral condition (p < 0.05). Results suggest that increased emotional distance is associated with decreased psychophysiologic concordance and reduced subjective ratings of perceived empathy. The observed differences in psychophysiologic concordance support the use of this measure as a potential marker of empathy in a clinical population in an interview setting.

KEYWORDS:

empathy emotional distance skin conductance psychophysiology gaze 

REFERENCES

  1. Arnetz, B. B. (2001). Psychosocial challenges facing physicians of today. Social Science and Medicine, 52, 203–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1962). Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(43), 1–36.Google Scholar
  3. Benbassat, J., & Baumal, R. (2004). What is empathy and how can it be promoted during clinical clerkships? Academic Medicine, 79(9), 832–839.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Berber, S. M. (1962). Conditioning through vicarious instigation. Psychological Review, 9(5), 450–466.Google Scholar
  5. Bohart, A. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (1997). Empathy and psychotherapy: An Introductory overview. In L. S. Greenberg (Ed.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions for psychotherapy (pp. 3–31). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  6. Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(1), 10–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1997). Communicative genes and the evolution of empathy. In W. J. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 17–43). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981). Attitudes and cognitive response: An electrophysiologic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2181–2199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Calia, R. (2004). ‘Only connect’: The centrality of doctor-patient relationships in primary care. Family Practice, 21(3), 232–233.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cape, J. (2001). Patient-rated therapeutic relationship and outcome in general practitioner treatment of psychological problems. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39(4), 383–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 100(9), 5497–5502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  14. Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner's behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 386–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Coleman, R., Greenblatt, M., & Solomon, H. C. (1956). Physiolgical evidence of rapport during psychotherapeutic interviews. Diseases of the Nervous System, 17, 71–77.Google Scholar
  16. Craig, K. D. (1968). Physiologic arousal as a function imagined, vicarious, and direct stress experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 513–520.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Craig, K. D., & Lowery, H. J. (1969). Heart-rate components of conditioned vicarious affective arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 381–387.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Critchley, H. D., Elliott, R., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(8), 3033–3040.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Crosbie, J. (1995). Interrupted time-series analysis with short series: Why it is problematic; how it can be improved. In J. M. Gottman (Ed.), The analysis of change (pp. 361–395). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  20. de Roten, Y., Fivaz-Depeursinge, E., Stern, D. J., Darwish, J., & Corboz-Warnery, A. (2000). Body and gaze formations and the communicational alliance in couple-therapist triads. Psychotherapy Research, 10(1), 30–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Di Mascio, A., Boyd, R. W., & Greenblatt, M. (1957). Physiological correlates of tension and antagonism during psychotherapy: A study of “interpersonal change”. Psychosomatic Medicine, 19(2), 99–104.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Di Mascio, A., Boyd, R. W., Greenblatt, M., & Solomon, H. C. (1955). The psychiatric interview: A sociophysiologic study. Diseases of the Nervous System, 16, 4–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Farrow, T., Zheng, Y., Wilkinson, I., Spence, S., Deakin, J., Tarrier, N., et al. (2001). Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport, 12, 2433–2438.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 7–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fredrickson, M., Furmark, T., Olsson, M. T., Fischer, H., Andersson, J., & Langstrom, B. (1998). Functional neuratomical correlates of electrodermal activity: A positron emission tomographic study. Psychophysiology, 35, 179–185.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hammond, D. C., Hepworth, D. H., & Smith, V. G. (2002). Improving therapeutic communication: A guide for developing effective techniques. San Francisco.Google Scholar
  28. Hugdahl, K. (1995). Electrodermal activity. In K. Hugdahl (Ed.), Psychophysiology: The mind-body perspective (pp. 101–130). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M., Rochat, P., et al. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research In Child Development, 66(2), 1–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kleinke, C. L., Staneski, R. A., & Berger, D. E. (1975). Evaluation of an interviewer as a function of interview gaze, reinforcement of subject gaze, and interviewer attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 115–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kohler, E. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Lacey, J. I. (1959). Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of pscyhotherapeutic process and outcome. In E. A. Rubinstein & M. B. Parloff (Eds.), Research in psychotherapy (pp. 160–208). Washington, DC: National Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  33. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interaction: physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 587–597.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1985). Physiological and affective predictors of change in relationship satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(1), 85–94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Levenson, R. W., & Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy: A physiological substrate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 234–246.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Levinson, W. (1994). Physician–patient communication: A key to malpractice prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1619–1620.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Lidberg, L., & Wallin, B. G. (1981). Sympathetic nerve discharges in relation to amplitude of skin resistance responses. Psychophysiology, 18, 268–270.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Malin, M., Hemminki, E., Raeikkoenen, O., Sihvo, S., & Peraelae, M. L. (2001). What do women want? Women's experiences of infertility treatment. Social Science and Medicine, 53(1), 123–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Marci, C. D., Ham, J., Moran, E. K., & Orr, S. P. (2006). Physiologic correlates of empathy and social-emotional process during psychotherapy. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  40. McCarron, L. T., & Appel, V. H. (1971). Categories of therapist verbalizations and patient-therapist autonomic response. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 37(1), 123–134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mechanic, D., McAlpine, D. D., & Rosenthal, M. (2001). Are patient's office visits with physicians getting shorter? New England Journal of Medicine, 344(3), 198–204.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Miller, G. (2005). Reflecting on another's mind: Mirror mechanisms built into the brain may help us understand each other. Science, 308, 945–947.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Mohr, D. (1995). Negative outcome in psychotherapy: A critical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 1–27.Google Scholar
  44. Nerdrum, P. (1997). Maintenance of the effect of training in communication skills: A controlled follow-up study of level of communicated empathy. British Journal of Social Work, 27(5), 705–722.Google Scholar
  45. Ochsner, K. N., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2001). A multiprocess perspective on the neuroscience of emotion. In G. A. Bonanno (Ed.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 38–81). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  46. Oppenheim, A. V., & Schafer, R. W. (1999). Discrete-time signal processing. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  47. Patterson, J. C., Ungerleider, L. G., & Bandettini, P. A. (2002). Task-independent functional brain activity correlation with skin conductance changes: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 17, 1797–1806.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Robinson, J. W., Herman, A., & Kaplan, B. J. (1982). Autonomic responses correlate with counselor-client empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29(2), 195–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Roessler, R., Bruch, H., Thum, L., & Collins, F. (1975). Physiologic correlates of affect during psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 29, 26–36.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Siepmann, M., Grossmann, J., Muck-Weymann, M., & Kirch, W. (2003). Effects of sertraline on autonomic and cognitive functions in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 168(3), 293–298.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory componenets of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1161.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Stanek, B., Hahn, P., & Mayer, H. (1973). Biometric findings on cardiac neurosis: Changes in ECG and heart rate in cardiopathic patients and their doctor during psychoanalytical initial interviews. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22, 289–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Strug, D., Ottman, R., Kaye, J., Saltzberg, S., Walker, J., & Mendez, H. (2003). Client satisfaction and staff empathy at pediatric HIV/AIDS programs. Journal of Social Service Research, 29(4), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sung, M., Marci, C. D., & Pentland, A. (2005). Wearable feedback systems for rehabilitation. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 2, 2–17.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Thorell, L. H., Kjellman, B., & D’Elia, G. (1987). Electrodermal activity in relation to diagnostic subgroups and symptoms of depressive patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76(6), 693–701.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Thorell, L. H., Kjellman, B. F., & d’Elia, G. (1987). Electrodermal activity in antidepressant medicated and unmedicated depressive patients and in matched healthy subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76, 684–692.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tronick, E. Z., Brushweiler-Stern, N., Harrison, A. M., Lyons-Ruth, K., et al. (1998). Dyadically expanded states of consciousness and the process of therapeutic change. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19(3), 290–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. van Dulmen, A. M., & Bensing, J. M. (2002). Health promoting effects of the physician–patient encounter. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 7(3), 289–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1980). Vicarious instigation and conditioning of facial expressive and autonomic responses to a model's expressive delay of pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(6), 909–923.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryMassachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical SchoolBostonUSA
  2. 2.Veterans Affairs Medical CenterManchesterUSA
  3. 3.Director of Social Neuroscience, Department of PsychiatryMassachusetts General HospitalBostonUSA

Personalised recommendations