Artificial Intelligence Review

, Volume 26, Issue 4, pp 255–268 | Cite as

Robots, insects and swarm intelligence

Article

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to consider the relationships between robots and insects. To this end, an overview is provided of the two main areas in which insects have been implicated in robotics research. First, robots have been used to provide working models of mechanisms underlying insect behaviour. Second, there are developments in robotics that have been inspired by our understanding of insect behaviour; in particular the approach of swarm robotics. In the final section of the paper, the possibility of achieving “strong swarm intelligence” is discussed. Two possible interpretations of strong swarm intelligence are raised: (1) the emergence of a group mind from a natural, or robot swarm, and (2) that behaviours could emerge from a swarm of artificial robots in the same way as they emerge from a biological swarm. Both interpretations are dismissed as being unachievable in principle. It is concluded that bio-robotic modelling and biological inspiration have made important contributions to both insect and robot research, but insects and robots remain separated by the divide between the living and the purely mechanical.

Keywords

Swarm intelligence Strong artificial intelligence Swarm robotics Bio-robotics Biological inspiration Social insects Emergence 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ampatzis C, Tuci W, Trianni V, Dorigo M (2007) Evolution of signalling in a group of robots controlled by dynamic neural networks. In: Sahin E, Spears WM, Winfield AFT (eds) Swarm robotics, LNCS 4433, Springer, pp 173–188Google Scholar
  2. Beckers R, Holland OE, Deneubourg JL (1994) From local actions to global tasks: stigmergy and collective robotics. In: Proceedings of A-Life IV, MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  3. Boden M (2000). Autopoiesis and life. Cogn Sci Q 1: 117–145 Google Scholar
  4. Bonabeau E, Dorigo M, Theraulaz G (1999) Swarm intelligence: from natural to artificial systems. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  5. Bradshaw EL and Bateson P (2000). Welfare implications of culling red deer (Cervus elaphus). Anim Welf 9: 3–24 Google Scholar
  6. Damasio A (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason and the human brain. Penguin BooksGoogle Scholar
  7. Damasio A (2000). The feeling of what happens: body, emotion and the making of consciousness. Vintage, London Google Scholar
  8. Dautenhahn K, Ogden B, Quick T (2002) From embodied to socially embedded agents—implications for interaction-aware robots. Cogn Syst Res 3(3):397–428. Special issue on Situated and Embodied Cognition, guest-editor: Tom Ziemke, ElsevierGoogle Scholar
  9. Deneubourg J-L, Goss S, Franks N, Sendova-Franks A, Detrain C and Chretien L (1991). The dynamics of collective sorting: robot-like ants and ant-like robots. In: Meyer, JA and Wilson, SW (eds) From animals to animats. Proceedings of the first international conference on simulation of adaptive behaviour, pp 356–363. MIT Press (A Bradford Book), Cambridge Google Scholar
  10. Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (1999). (eds) Information processing in social insects. Birkhauser Verlag, BaselGoogle Scholar
  11. Franceschini N, Pichon JM and Blanes C (1992). From insect vision to robot vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond 337: 283–294 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frisch K von (1954) The dancing bees: an account of the life and senses of the honey bee (tr Dora Illse). Methuen and Company, Ltd, London, xiv + 183 50 ppGoogle Scholar
  13. Fong T, Nourbakhsh I and Dautenhahn K (2003). A survey of socially interactive robots. Rob Auton Syst 42: 143–166 MATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Grassé PP (1959). La reconstruction du nid et les coordinations inter-individuelles chez Bellicositermes Natalensis et Cubitermes sp La théorie de la stigmergie: essai d’interprétation du comportement des termites constructeur. Insectes Sociaux 6: 41–80 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Grasso F, Consi, T, Mountain D, Aterna J (1996) Locating odor sources in turbulence with a lobster inspired robot. In: Proceedings of the fourth international conference on simulation of adaptive behaviour, From animals to animats, (SAB96), Cape Cod, MA, pp 104–112Google Scholar
  16. Griffin DR (1992). Animal minds. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago Google Scholar
  17. Harnad S (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42: 335–346 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hedwig B and Poulet JFP (2004). Complex auditory behaviour emerges from simple reactive steering. Nature 430: 781–785 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Holland O and Melhuish C (1999). Stigmergy, self-organisation and sorting in collective robotics. Artif Life 5: 173–202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ijspeert AJ, Martinoli A, Billard A and Gambardella LM (2001). Collaboration through the exploitation of local interactions in autonomous collective robotics: the stick pulling experiment. Auton Robots 11: 149–171 MATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jackson DE and Ratnieks FLW (2006). Communication in ants. Curr Biol 16(15): 570–574 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kelly K (1995) Out of control: the new biology of machines, social systems and the economic world. Perseus BooksGoogle Scholar
  23. Kube CR and Bonabeau E (2000). Cooperative transport by ants and robots. Rob Auton Syst 30: 85–101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kube C, Zhang H (1996) The use of perceptual cues in multi-robot box-pushing. In: Proceedings of IEEE international conference on robotics and automation, pp 2085–2090Google Scholar
  25. Labhart T(1988) Unpublished resultsGoogle Scholar
  26. Labhart T (1998). Polarization-opponent interneurons in the insect visual system. Nature 331: 435–437 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lambrinos D, Maris M, Kobayashi H, Labhart T, Pfeifer R and Wehner R (1997). An autonomous agent navigating with a polarized light compass. Adap Behav 6(1): 131–161 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lambrinos D, Moller R, Labhart T, Pfeifer R and Wehner R (2000). A mobile robot employing insect strategies for navigation. Rob Auton Syst 30: 39–64 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Loeb J (1912). The mechanistic conception of life: biological essays. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago Google Scholar
  30. Loeb J (1918). Forced movements, tropisms and animal conduct. Lippincott Company, Philadelphia Google Scholar
  31. Lund H, Webb B and Hallam J (1998). Physical and temporal scaling: considerations in a robot model of cricket calling song preference. Artif Life 4: 95–107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Marocco D and Nolfi S (2007). Emergence of communication in embodied agents for the ability to solve a collective navigation problem. Connect Sci 19(1): 53–74 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mallon EB, Pratt SC and Franks NR (2001). Individual and collective decision-making during nest site selection by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50: 353–359 Google Scholar
  34. Maturana HR and Varela FJ (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: the realization of the living. Reidel, Boston Google Scholar
  35. Moser JC (1970) Pheromones of social insects. In: Wood D, Silverstein R, Nakajima M (eds) Control of insect behavior by natural products. Academic Press, pp 161–178Google Scholar
  36. Nolfi S and Floreano D (2000). Evolutionary robotics: the biology, intelligence and technology of self-organizing machines. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge Google Scholar
  37. Noth W (2005). Semiotics for biologists. J Biosemiotics 1: 195–211 Google Scholar
  38. Papaj DR and Lewis AC (1992). (eds) Insect learning: ecological and evolutionary perspectives. Chapman and Hall, New York Google Scholar
  39. Pratt SC, Mallon EB, Sumpter DB and Franks NR (2002). Quorum-dependent recruitment and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52(2): 117–127 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Quick T, Dautenhahn K (1999) Making embodiment measurable, Workshop Contribution “Embodied Mind/Artificial Life” at 4. Fachtagung der Gesellschaft für Kognitionswissenschaft, 28. September - 1. Oktober 1999 in Bielefeld (KogWis99)Google Scholar
  41. Ronacher B and Wehner R (1999). The individual at the core of information management. In: Detrain, C, Deneubourg, JL and Pasteels, JM (eds) Information processing in social insects, pp. Birkhauser Verlag, Basel Google Scholar
  42. Schildberger K (1984). Temporal selectivity of identified auditory interneurons in the cricket brain. J Comp Physiol A 155: 171–185 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Searle JR (1980). Minds, brains and programs. Behav Brain Sci 3: 417–457 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Searle JR (1997). The mystery of consciousness. Granta Books, London Google Scholar
  45. Sebeok TA (1965). Animal communication. Science 147: 1006–1013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sharkey AJC (2007). Swarm robotics and minimalism. Connect Sci 19(3): 245–260 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sharkey AJC, Sharkey NE (2002) Cognitive modelling: psychology and connectionism. In: Arbib MA (ed) The handbook of brain theory and neural networks, 2nd ed. Bradford Books/MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  48. Sharkey NE and Ziemke T (2000). Life, mind and robots—the ins and outs of embodied cognition. In: Wermter, S and Sun, R (eds) Hybrid neural systems, pp. Springer Verlag, Heidelberg Google Scholar
  49. Sharkey NE and Ziemke T (2001). Mechanistic vs phenomenal embodiment: can robot embodiment lead to strong AI. Cogn Syst Res 2(4): 251–262 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sherrington CS (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. C.Scribner’s Sons, New York Google Scholar
  51. Srinivasan MV, Chahl JS and Zhang SW (1997). Robot navigation by visual dead-reckoning: inspiration from insects. Intern J Pattern Recognit Artif Intell 11(1): 35–47 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tan KC, Wang LF, Lee TH and Vadakkepat P (2004). Evolvable hardware in evolutionary robotics. Auton Robots 16: 5–21 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Visscher PK and Camazine S (1999). The mystery of swarming honeybees: from individual behaviours to collective decisions. In: Detrain, C, Deneubourg, JL and Pasteels, JM (eds) Information processing in social insects, pp. Birkhauser Verlag, Basel Google Scholar
  54. Vitzthum H (1997) Der Zentralkomplex der Heuschrecke Schistocera gregaria: Ein mogliches Zentrum des Polarisationssehsystems. PhD thesis, University RegensburgGoogle Scholar
  55. Webb B (1995). Using robots to model animals: a cricket test. Rob Auton Syst 16: 117–134 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Webb B (2001). Can robots make good models of biological behaviour?   . Behav Brain Sci 24(6): 1033–1050 Google Scholar
  57. Webb B and Scutt T (2000). A simple latency dependent spiking neuron model of cricket phonotaxis. Biol Cybern 82: 247–269 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wehner R and Wehner S (1986). Path integration in desert ants: approaching a long-standing puzzle in insect navigation. Monitore Zool Ital (N.S.) 20: 309–331 Google Scholar
  59. Wenner AM and Wells PH (1990). Anatomy of a controversy: the question of a “Language” among bees. Columbia University Press, New York Google Scholar
  60. Wilson M, Melhuish C, Sendova-Franks AB and Scholes S (2004). Algorithms for building annular structures with minimalist robots inspired by brood sorting in ant colonies. Auton Rob 17: 115–136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Computer ScienceUniversity of Sheffield Regent CourtSheffieldUK

Personalised recommendations