Advertisement

Cognitive Processing

, Volume 15, Issue 3, pp 343–349 | Cite as

Reading about explanations enhances perceptions of inevitability and foreseeability: a cross-cultural study with Wikipedia articles

  • Aileen OeberstEmail author
  • Ina von der Beck
  • Steffen Nestler
Research Report

Abstract

In hindsight, people often perceive events to be more inevitable and foreseeable than in foresight. According to Causal Model Theory (Nestler et al. in J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34: 1043–1054, 2008), causal explanations are crucial for such hindsight distortions to occur. The present study provides further empirical support for this notion but extends previous findings in several ways. First, ecologically valid materials were used. Second, the effect of causal information on hindsight distortions was investigated in the realm of previously known events. Third, cross-cultural differences in reasoning (analytic vs. holistic) were taken into account. Specifically, German and Vietnamese participants in our study were presented with Wikipedia articles about the nuclear power plant in Fukushima Daiichi, Japan. They read either the version that existed before the nuclear disaster unfolded (Version 1) or the article that existed 8 weeks after the catastrophe commenced (Version 2). Only the latter contained elaborations on causal antecedents and therefore provided an explanation for the disaster. Reading that version led participants to perceive the nuclear disaster to be more likely inevitable and foreseeable when compared to reading Version 1. Cultural background did not exert a significant effect on these perceptions. Hence, hindsight distortions were obtained for ecologically valid materials even if the event was already known. Implications and directions for future research are discussed.

Keywords

Hindsight bias Causal models Wikipedia Cross-cultural comparisons 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Preparation of the manuscript was supported by grants from the DFG to Aileen Oeberst and Ulrike Cress (OE 604/1-1) and Steffen Nestler and Mitja D. Back (NE 1485/5-1). We are grateful to Nguyen Thi Hoai Nga who helped us with collecting data from Vietnamese students.

References

  1. Anderson JC, Jennings MM, Lowe DJ, Reckers PMJ (1997) The mitigation of hindsight bias in judges’ evaluation of auditor decisions. Audit J Pract Theory 16:20–39Google Scholar
  2. Arkes HR, Faust D, Guilmette TJ, Hart K (1988) Eliminating the hindsight bias. J Appl Psychol 73:305–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ash IK (2009) Surprise, memory, and retrospective judgment making: testing cognitive reconstruction theories of the hindsight bias effect. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35:916–933PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blank H, Nestler S (2007) Cognitive process models of hindsight bias. Soc Cognit 25:132–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blank H, Nestler S, von Collani G, Fischer V (2008) How many hindsight biases are there? Cognition 106:1408–1440PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boroditsky L, Fuhrman O, McCormick K (2011) Do English and mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition 118:123–129PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Calvillo DP, Gomes DM (2011) Surprise influences hindsight–foresight differences in temporal judgments of animated automobile accidents. Psychon Bull Rev 18:385–391PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carli LL (1999) Cognitive reconstruction, hindsight, and reactions to victims and perpetrators. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 25:966–979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Choi I, Nisbett RE (2000) Cultural psychology of surprise: holistic theories and recognition of contradiction. J Pers Soc Psychol 79:890–905PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Choi I, Dalal R, Kim-Prieto C, Park H (2003) Culture and judgment of causal relevance. J Pers Soc Psychol 84:46–59PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Choi I, Koo M, Choi JA (2007) Individual differences in analytic versus holistic thinking. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 33:691–705PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dawes RM (1993) Prediction of the future versus an understanding of the past: a basic asymmetry. Am J Psychol 106:1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fischhoff B (1977) Perceived informativeness of facts. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 3:349–358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Forte A, Bruckman A (2008). Scaling consensus: increasing decentralization in Wikipedia governance. Proceedings of the international conference of the learning sciences, Bloomington, USAGoogle Scholar
  15. Giles J (2005) Internet encyclopaedias go head to head. Nature 438:900–901PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Guilbault RL, Bryant FB, Brockway JH, Posavac EJ (2004) A meta-analysis of research on hindsight bias. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 26:103–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hastie R, Schkade DA, Payne JW (1999) Juror judgments in civil cases: hindsight effects on judgments of liability for punitive damages. Law Hum Behav 23:597–614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Heine SJ, Lehman DR (1996) Hindsight bias: a cross-cultural analysis. Jpn J Exp Soc Psychol 35:317–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Keegan B, Gergle D, Contractor N (2011) Hot off the Wiki: dynamics, practices, and structures in Wikipedia’s coverage of the Tōhoku catastrophes. Proceedings of the 7th international symposium on wikis and open collaboration, ACM Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Kittur A, Kraut R (2008) Harnessing the wisdom of crowds in Wikipedia: quality through coordination. Proceedings of the ACM 2008 conference on computer supported cooperative work, ACM Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Louie TA (2005) Hindsight bias and outcome-consistent thoughts when observing and making service provider decisions. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 98:88–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Merz M, Döring N (2010) Aktive beteiligung an Wikipedia aus sozial-kognitiver perspektive. http://purl.org/merz/20100926 on 27 Nov 2012
  23. Miller JG (1984) Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. J Pers Soc Psychol 46:961–978PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nario MR, Branscombe NR (1995) Comparison processes in hindsight and causal attributions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 21:1244–1255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nestler S, Egloff B (2009) Increased or reversed? The effect of surprise on hindsight bias depends on the hindsight component. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35:1539–1544PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Nestler S, von Collani G (2008) Hindsight bias and the activation of counterfactual mind-sets. Exp Psychol 55:342–349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nestler S, Blank H, von Collani G (2008) Hindsight bias doesn’t always come easy: causal models, cognitive effort and creeping determinism. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34:1043–1054PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nestler S, Blank H, Egloff B (2010) Hindsight ≠ Hindsight: experimentally induced dissociations between hindsight components. J Exp Psychol Lern Mem Cogn 36:1399–1413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Nisbett RE (2003) The geography of thought. Free Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  30. Nisbett RE, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A (2001) Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol Rev 108:291–310PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Norenzayan A, Smith EE, Kim BJ, Nisbett RE (2002) Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cogn Sci 26:653–684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Norenzayan A, Choi I, Peng K (2007) Perception and cognition. In: Kitayama S, Cohen D (eds) Handbook of cultural psychology. Guilford Press, New York, pp 569–594Google Scholar
  33. Peng K, Nisbett RE (1999) Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. Am Psychol 54:741–754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pezzo MV (2003) Surprise, defense, or making sense: what removes the hindsight bias? Memory 11:421–441PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pezzo MV (2011) Hindsight bias: a primer for motivational researchers. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 5:665–678CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pohl RF, Bender M, Lachmann G (2002) Hindsight bias around the world. Exp Psychol 49:270–282PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Roese NJ, Olson JM (1996) Counterfactuals, causal attributions, and the hindsight bias: a conceptual integration. J Exp Soc Psychol 32:197–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Roese NJ, Vohs KD (2012) Hindsight bias. Perspect Psychol Sci 7:411–426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ross M, Xun WQ, Wilson AE (2002) Language and the bicultural self. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 28:1040–1050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Schkade DA, Kilbourne LM (1991) Expectation-outcome consistency and hindsight bias. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 49:105–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Oeberst A, Halatchliyski I, Kimmerle J, Cress U (2014) Knowledge construction in Wikipedia: a systemic-constructivist analysis. J Learn Sci (in press). http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/10508406.2014.888352
  42. Yama H, Manktelow KI, Mercier H, van der Henst JB, Soo Do K, Kawasaki Y, Adachi K (2010) A cross-cultural study of hindsight bias and conditional probabilistic reasoning. Think Reason 16:346–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Yopchick JE, Kim NS (2012) Hindsight bias and causal reasoning: a minimalist approach. Cogn Process 13:63–72PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aileen Oeberst
    • 1
    Email author
  • Ina von der Beck
    • 1
  • Steffen Nestler
    • 2
  1. 1.Knowledge Media Research CenterTübingenGermany
  2. 2.University of MünsterMünsterGermany

Personalised recommendations