Cognitive Processing

, Volume 15, Issue 2, pp 209–215 | Cite as

Emotion words and categories: evidence from lexical decision

  • Graham G. Scott
  • Patrick J. O’Donnell
  • Sara C. Sereno
Short Report

Abstract

We examined the categorical nature of emotion word recognition. Positive, negative, and neutral words were presented in lexical decision tasks. Word frequency was additionally manipulated. In Experiment 1, “positive” and “negative” categories of words were implicitly indicated by the blocked design employed. A significant emotion–frequency interaction was obtained, replicating past research. While positive words consistently elicited faster responses than neutral words, only low frequency negative words demonstrated a similar advantage. In Experiments 2a and 2b, explicit categories (“positive,” “negative,” and “household” items) were specified to participants. Positive words again elicited faster responses than did neutral words. Responses to negative words, however, were no different than those to neutral words, regardless of their frequency. The overall pattern of effects indicates that positive words are always facilitated, frequency plays a greater role in the recognition of negative words, and a “negative” category represents a somewhat disparate set of emotions. These results support the notion that emotion word processing may be moderated by distinct systems.

Keywords

Emotion Word frequency Category Lexical decision Arousal Valence 

References

  1. Balota DA, Yap MJ, Cortese MJ, Hutchison KA, Kessler B, Loftis B, Neely JH, Nelson DL, Simpson GB, Treiman R (2007) The English Lexicon Project. Behav Res Methods 39(3):445–459Google Scholar
  2. Barsalou LW (1983) Ad hoc categories. Mem Cogn 11(3):211–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bazzanella B, Bouquet P (2011) Associative and categorical priming in recognition of individuals. In: Carlson L, Hölscher C, Shipley TF (eds) Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA, pp 525–530Google Scholar
  4. Becker CA (1979) Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 5(2):252–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Becker CA (1980) Semantic context effects in visual word recognition: an analysis of semantic strategies. Mem Cogn 8(6):493–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bermeitinger C, Wentura D, Frings C (2011) How to switch on and switch off semantic priming effects for natural and artifactual categories: activation processes in category memory depend on focusing specific feature dimensions. Psychon Bull Rev 18(3):579–585PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Borowsky R, Besner D (2006) Parallel distributed processing and lexical-semantic effects in visual word recognition: are a few stages necessary? Psychol Rev 113(1):181–195PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1999) Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual, and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainsville, FLGoogle Scholar
  9. Briesemeister BB, Kuchinke L, Jacobs AM (2011a) Discrete emotion effects on lexical decision response times. PLoS ONE 6(8):e23743PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Briesemeister BB, Kuchinke L, Jacobs AM (2011b) Discrete Emotion Norms for Nouns: Berlin Affective Word List (DENN-BAWL). Behav Res Methods 43(2):441–448PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ekman P, Friesen WV (1971) Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol 17(2):124–129PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Estes Z, Adelman JS (2008a) Automatic vigilance for negative words in lexical decision and naming: comment on Larsen, Mercer, and Balota (2006). Emotion 8(4):441–444PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Estes Z, Adelman JS (2008b) Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general. Emotion 8(4):453–457CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fontaine JRJ, Scherer KR, Roesch EB, Ellsworth PC (2007) The world of emotion is not two-dimensional. Psychol Sci 18(12):1050–1057PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hand CJ, Miellet S, O’Donnell PJ, Sereno SC (2010) The frequency-predictability interaction in reading: it depends where you’re coming from. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36(5):1294–1313PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hauk O, Davis MH, Ford M, Pulvermüller F, Marslen-Wilson WD (2006) The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. NeuroImage 30(4):1383–1400PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kousta S-T, Vinson DP, Vigliocco G (2009) Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition 112(3):473–481PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kuchinke L, Võ ML, Hofmann M, Jacobs AM (2007) Pupillary responses during lexical decisions vary with word frequency but not emotional valence. Int J Psychophysiol 65(2):132–140PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Larsen RJ, Mercer KA, Balota DA, Strube MJ (2008) Not all negative words slow down lexical decision and naming speed: importance of word arousal. Emotion 8(4):445–452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lerner JS, Keltner D (2000) Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cogn Emot 14(4):473–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McGinnies E (1949) Emotionality and perceptual defense. Psychol Rev 56(5):244–251PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Méndez-Bértolo C, Pozo JA, Hinojosa JA (2011) Word frequency modulates the processing of emotional words: convergent behavioral and electrophysiological data. Neurosci Lett 494(3):250–254PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nakic M, Smith BW, Busis S, Vythilingam M, Blair RJR (2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: the role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage 31(4):1752–1761PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Pratto F, John OP (1991) Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. J Pers Soc Psychol 61(3):380–391PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Russell JA (1980) A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 39(6):1161–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Sachs O, Weis S, Zellagui N, Huber W, Zvyaginstev M, Mathiak K, Kircher T (2008) Automatic processing of semantic relations in fMRI: neuronal activation during semantic priming of taxonomic and thematic categories. Brain Res 1218:194–205PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schacter DL, Badgaiyan RD (2001) Neuroimaging of priming: new perspectives on implicit and explicit memory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 10(1):1–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Scott GG, O’Donnell PJ, Leuthold H, Sereno SC (2009) Early emotion word processing: evidence from event-related potentials. Biol Psychol 80(1):95–104PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Scott GG, O’Donnell PJ, Sereno SC (2012) Emotion words affect eye fixations during reading. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 38(3):783–792PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Segalowitz SJ, Zheng X (2009) An ERP study of category priming: evidence of early lexical semantic access. Biol Psychol 80(1):122–129PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Sereno SC, Rayner K (2003) Measuring word recognition in reading: eye movements and event-related potentials. Trends Cogn Sci 7(11):489–493PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Stevenson RA, Mikels JA, James TW (2007) Characterization of the Affective Norms for English Words by discrete emotional categories. Behav Res Methods 39(4):1020–1024PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Taylor SE (1991) Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychol Bull 110(1):67–85PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Unkelbach C, Fiedler K, Bayer M, Stegmüller M, Danner D (2008) Why positive information is processed faster: the density hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol 95(1):36–49PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Wurm LH (2007) Danger and usefulness: an alternative framework for understanding rapid evaluation effects in perception? Psychon Bull Rev 14(6):1218–1225PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Graham G. Scott
    • 1
  • Patrick J. O’Donnell
    • 2
  • Sara C. Sereno
    • 2
  1. 1.Division of Psychology, School of Social SciencesUniversity of the West of ScotlandPaisleyUK
  2. 2.Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, School of PsychologyUniversity of GlasgowGlasgowUK

Personalised recommendations