Advertisement

Cognitive Processing

, Volume 15, Issue 2, pp 159–172 | Cite as

The Bergen left–right discrimination test: practice effects, reliable change indices, and strategic performance in the standard and alternate form with inverted stimuli

  • Philip Grewe
  • Hanno A. Ohmann
  • Hans J. Markowitsch
  • Martina Piefke
Research Report

Abstract

Several authors pointed out that left–right discrimination (LRD) tasks may be entangled with differential demands on mental rotation (MR). However, studies considering this interrelationship are rare. To differentially assess LRD of stimuli with varying additional demands on MR, we constructed and evaluated an extended version of the Bergen right–left discrimination (BRLD) test including additional subtests with inverted stickmen stimuli in 174 healthy participants (50♂, 124♀) and measured subjective reports on participants’ strategies to accomplish the task. Moreover, we analyzed practice effects and reliable change indices (RCIs) on BRLD performance, as well as gender differences. Performance significantly differed between subtests with high and low demands on MR with best scores on subtests with low demands on MR. Participants’ subjective strategies corroborate these results: MR was most frequently reported for subtests with highest MR demands (and lowest test performance). Pronounced practice effects were observed for all subtests. Sex differences were not observed. We conclude that our extended version of the BRLD allows for the differentiation between LRD with high and low demands on MR abilities. The type of stimulus materials is likely to be critical for the differential assessment of MR and LRD. Moreover, RCIs provide a basis for the clinical application of the BRLD.

Keywords

Left–right discrimination Mental rotation Practice effects Spatial cognition Subjective strategies 

Notes

Acknowledgments

MP and HJM are supported by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft [DFG]; EC 277). We are very thankful to Sonja Ofte for the stimuli of the Bergen right–left discrimination test. We thank our colleagues in the Physiological Psychology department and the CITmed project for their scientific and technical support and helpful advice. We especially thank Alsonso Ortega for his helpful advice concerning the statistical analyses.

Supplementary material

10339_2013_587_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (17 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (PDF 17 kb)
10339_2013_587_MOESM2_ESM.pdf (21 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (PDF 21 kb)
10339_2013_587_MOESM3_ESM.pdf (47 kb)
Supplementary material 3 (PDF 46 kb)

References

  1. Auer T, Schwarcz A, Aradi M, Kalmár Z, Pendleton C, Janszky I, Horváth RA, Szalay C, Dóczi T, Komoly S (2008) Right–left discrimination is related to the right hemisphere. Laterality 13(5):427–438PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baenninger M, Newcombe N (1989) The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles 20(5):327–344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Band GPH, Kok A (2000) Age effects on response monitoring in a mental-rotation task. Biol Psychol 51(2–3):201–221PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Benton AL (1959) Right–left discrimination and finger localization: development and pathology. Hoeber, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Benton A (1968) Right–left discrimination. Pediatr Clin N Am 15(3):747–758Google Scholar
  6. Berg C, Hertzog C, Hunt E (1982) Age differences in the speed of mental rotation. Dev Psychol 18(1):95–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brandt MG, Davies ET (2006) Visual-spatial ability, learning modality and surgical knot tying. Can J Surg 49(6):412–416PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cermak S (1984) Right–left discrimination in learning disabled and normal control boys. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 4(2):63–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chelune GJ, Naugle RI, Lüders H, Sedlak J, Awad IA (1993) Individual change after epilepsy surgery: practice effects and base-rate information. Neuropsychology 7(1):41–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Corballis MC, Cullen S (1986) Decisions about the axes of disoriented shapes. Mem Cognit 14(1):27–38PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Corballis MC, McMaster H (1996) The roles of stimulus-response compatibility and mental rotation in mirror-image and left–right decisions. Can J Exp Psychol 50(4):397–401PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dalecki M, Hoffmann U, Bock O (2012) Mental rotation of letters, body parts and complex scenes: separate or common mechanisms? Hum Mov Sci 31(5):1151–1160PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gardner MR, Potts R (2010) Hand dominance influences the processing of observed bodies. Brain Cognit 73(1):35–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gormley GJ, Dempster M, Best R (2008) Right–left discrimination among medical students: questionnaire and psychometric study. Br Med J 337:a2826CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Guillot A, Champely S, Batier C, Thiriet P, Collet C (2007) Relationship between spatial abilities, mental rotation and functional anatomy learning. Adv Health Sci Edu 12(4):491–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Harris IM, Harris JA, Caine D (2002) Mental-rotation deficits following damage to the right basal ganglia. Neuropsychology 16(4):524–537PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Heaton RK, Temkin N, Dikmen S, Avitable N, Taylor MJ, Marcotte TD, Grant I (2001) Detecting change: a comparison of three neuropsychological methods, using normal and clinical samples. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 16(1):75–91PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Heil M, Rösler F, Link M, Bajric J (1998) What is improved if a mental rotation task is repeated–the efficiency of memory access, or the speed of a transformation routine? Psychol Res 61(2):99–106PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hirnstein M, Bayer U, Ellison A, Hausmann M (2011) TMS over the left angular gyrus impairs the ability to discriminate left from right. Neuropsychologia 49(1):29–33PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hoyek N, Collet C, Rastello O, Fargier P, Thiriet P, Guillot A (2009) Enhancement of mental rotation abilities and its effect on anatomy learning. Teach Learn Med 21(3):201–206PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jansen-Osmann P, Heil M (2007) Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain Cognit 64(3):217–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jordan K, Wüstenberg T, Jaspers-Feyer F, Fellbrich A, Peters M (2006) Sex differences in left/right confusion. Cortex 42(1):69–78PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kali Y, Orion N (1996) Spatial abilities of high-school students in the perception of geologic structures. J Res Sci Teach 33(4):369–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kirasic KC (2000) Age differences in adults’ spatial abilities, learning environmental layout, and wayfinding behavior. Sp Cognit Comput 2(2):117–134Google Scholar
  25. Kozhevnikov M, Kosslyn S, Shephard J (2005) Spatial versus object visualizers: a new characterization of visual cognitive style. Mem Cognit 33(4):710–726PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lord SAG, Marsh GR (1975) Age differences in the speed of a spatial cognitive process. J Gerontol 30(6):674–678CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Maassen GH, Bossema E, Brand N (2009) Reliable change and practice effects: outcomes of various indices compared. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 31(3):339–352PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Malinowski JC (2001) Mental rotation and real-world wayfinding. Percept Mot Skills 92(1):19–30PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McCaffrey RJ, Westervelt H (1995) Issues associated with repeated neuropsychological assessments. Neuropsychol Rev 5(3):203–221PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. McCaffrey RJ, Ortega A, Orsillo SM, Nelles WB, Haase RF (1992) Practice effects in repeated neuropsychological assessments. Clin Neuropsychol 6(1):32–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Newcombe NS, Frick A (2010) Early education for spatial intelligence: why, what, and how. Mind Brain Educ 4(3):102–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ocklenburg S, Hirnstein M, Ohmann HA, Hausmann M (2011) Mental rotation does not account for sex differences in left–right confusion. Brain Cognit 76(1):166–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ofte SH (2002) Right–left discrimination: effects of handedness and educational background. Scand J Psychol 43(3):213–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ofte SH, Hugdahl K (2002a) Right–left discrimination in younger and older children measured with two tests containing stimuli on different abstraction levels. Percept Mot Skills 94(3):707–719PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ofte SH, Hugdahl K (2002b) Rightleft discrimination in male and female, young and old subjects. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24(1):82–92PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Parsons LM (1987a) Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. J Exp Psychol Gen 116(2):172–191PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Parsons LM (1987b) Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognit Psychol 19(2):178–241PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Parsons TD, Notebaert AJ, Shields EW, Guskiewicz KM (2009) Application of reliable change indices to computerized neuropsychological measures of concussion. Int J Neurosci 119(4):492–507PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Peters M, Laeng B, Latham K, Jackson M, Zaiyouna R, Richardson C (1995) A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test-different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain Cognit 28(1):39–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1948) La représentation de l’espace chez l’enfant. Presses Universitaires de Frances, ParisGoogle Scholar
  41. Ratcliff G (1979) Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 17(1):49–54PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rigal R (1996) Right–left orientation, mental rotation, and perspective-taking: when can children imagine what people see from their viewpoint? Percept Mot Skills 83(3):831–842PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shepard RN, Hurwitz S (1984) Upward direction, mental rotation, and discrimination of left and right turns in maps. Cognit 18(1–3):161–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Shepard RN, Metzler J (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Snyder TJ (1991) Self-rated right–left confusability and objectively measured right–left discrimination. Dev Neuropsychol 7(2):219–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Terlecki MS, Newcombe NS, Little M (2008) Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: gender differences in growth patterns. Appl Cogn Psychol 22(7):996–1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7(1):38–42PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Voyer D (1995) Effect of practice on laterality in a mental rotation task. Brain Cognit 29(3):326–335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP (2009) Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol 101(4):817–835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wright R, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Newcombe NS, Kosslyn SM (2008) Training generalized spatial skills. Psychonomic Bull Rev 15(4):763–771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Zacks JM (2008) Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: a meta-analysis and review. J Cognit Neurosci 20(1):1–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Zacks JM, Mires J, Tversky B, Hazeltine E (2000) Mental spatial transformations of objects and perspective. Spatial Cognit Comput 2(4):315–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Philip Grewe
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Hanno A. Ohmann
    • 2
    • 4
  • Hans J. Markowitsch
    • 1
    • 2
  • Martina Piefke
    • 1
    • 2
    • 5
  1. 1.Physiological PsychologyBielefeld UniversityBielefeldGermany
  2. 2.Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology (CITEC)Bielefeld UniversityBielefeldGermany
  3. 3.Bethel Epilepsy CentreMara HospitalBielefeldGermany
  4. 4.Department of Research, Evaluation and Documentation, Clinic of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy BethelEv. Hospital BielefeldBielefeldGermany
  5. 5.Neurobiology and Genetics of BehaviorWitten/Herdecke UniversityWittenGermany

Personalised recommendations