How to put things together
- 509 Downloads
- 8 Citations
Abstract
Instructions for putting things together or understanding how things work are notoriously frustrating. Performance relies on constructing mental models of the object and the actions of the object from text or diagrams or both. Here, we show that instructions can be improved by turning users into designers and deriving design principles from their designs. People first assembled an object and then crafted assembly instructions, using text alone or text and diagrams. Some were required to be brief and to include only the most essential information. Users’ instructions had a narrative structure with an introduction, a middle, and an end. The essential middle described or depicted the step-by-step sequence of actions on parts. Diagrams were regarded as fundamental, and redundancy of depictions and descriptions desirable. These design principles have wide applicability to many kinds of explanations.
Keywords
Assembly instructions Diagrams Spatial abilityNotes
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Julie Heiser for her participation in the design, execution, and analyses of the second experiment. The research was aided by the following grants: National Science Foundation HHC 0905417, IIS-0725223, IIS-0855995, and REC 0440103, the Stanford Regional Visualization and Analysis Center, and Office of Naval Research NOOO14-PP-1-O649, N000140110717, and N000140210534.
References
- Bauer MI, Johnson-Laird PN (1993) How diagrams can improve reasoning. Psychol Sci 4(6):372–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Carroll JM, Mack RL, Robertson SP, Rosson MB (1994) Binding objects to scenarios of use. Int J Hum Comput Stud 41:243–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cheng PC-H (1996) Functional roles for the cognitive analysis of diagrams in problem solving. In: Cottrell GW (ed) Proceeding of the eighteenth annual conference of the cognitive science society. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 207–212Google Scholar
- Daniel M-P, Denis M (2003) The production of route directions: investigating conditions that favor concise spatial discourse. Appl Cogn Psychol 18(1):57–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Daniel MP, Manghi E, Tom A, Denis M (2003) Testing the value of route directions through navigational performance. Spat Cogn Comput 3(4):269–289Google Scholar
- Denis M (1997) The description of routes: a cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse. Curr Psychol Cogn 16:409–458Google Scholar
- Dixon P (1987a) The processing of mental plans for following directions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 13(1):18–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dixon P (1987b) The processing of organizational and component step information in written directions. J Mem Lang 26:24–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ganier F (2004) Factors affecting the processing of procedural instructions: implications for document design. IEEE Trans Prof Commun 47(1):15–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gentner D, Stevens AL (1983) Mental models. Erlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
- Glenberg AM, Langston WE (1992) Comprehension of illustrated text: pictures help to build mental models. J Mem Lang 31:129–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hegarty M, Carpenter PA, Just MA (1990) Diagrams in the comprehension of scientific text. In: Barr R, Kamil ML, Mosenthal P, Pearson PD (eds) Handbook of reading research. Longman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
- Heiser J, Phan D, Agrawala M, Tversky B, Hanrahan P (2004) Identification and validation of cognitive design principles for automated generation of assembly instructions. In: Proceedings of advanced visual interfaces’04, pp 311–319, ACMGoogle Scholar
- Kessell AM, Tversky B (2011) Visualizing space, time, and agents: production, performance, and preference. Cogn Process 12:43–52. doi: 10.1007/s10339-010-0379-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Krull R, D’Souza S, Roy D, Sharp DM, Roy D (2004) Designing procedural illustrations: IEEE transactions on professional communication. Acquir Proced Knowl Technol Interface 47(1):27–33Google Scholar
- Larkin JH, Simon HA (1987) Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cogn Sci 11(1):65–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Linn MC, Petersen AC (1986) A meta-anaysis of gender differences in spatial ability: Implications for mathematics and science achievement. In: Hyde JS, Linn MC (eds) The psychology of gender: advances through metaanalysis. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp 67–101Google Scholar
- Mani K, Johnson-Laird PN (1982) The mental representation of spatial descriptions. Memory Cogn 10:181–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Marcus N, Cooper M, Sweller J (1996) Understanding instructions. J Educ Psychol 88:49–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mijksenaar P, Westendorp P (1999) Open here: the art of instructional design. Thames and Hudson, LondonGoogle Scholar
- Norman DA (1998) The design of everyday things. Doubleday, New YorkGoogle Scholar
- Novick LR (2001) Spatial diagrams: key instruments in the toolbox for thought. In: Medin DL (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 40. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 279–325Google Scholar
- Novick LR (2006) Understanding spatial diagram structure. Q J Exp Psychol 59:1826–1856CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Novick LR, Morse DL (2000) Folding a fish, making a mushroom: the role of diagrams in executing assembly procedures. Memory Cogn 28(7):1242–1256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Scaife M, Rogers Y (1996) External cognition: how do graphical representations work? Int J Hum Comput Stud 45:185–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schnotz W (2002) Towards an integrated view of learning from text and visual displays. Educ Psychol Rev 14(1):101–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stenning K, Oberlander J (1995) A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: logic and implementation. Cogn Sci 19:97–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tufte ER (1983) The visual display of quantitative information. Graphics Press, CheshireGoogle Scholar
- Tversky B (2001) Spatial schemas in depictions. In: Gattis M (ed) Spatial schemas and abstract thought. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 79–111Google Scholar
- Tversky B (2005) Visualspatial reasoning. In: Holyoak K, Morrison R (eds) Handbook of reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 209–249Google Scholar
- Tversky B (2011) Visualizations of thought. Top Cogn Sci 3:499–535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tversky B, Agrawala M, Heiser J, Lee PU, Hanrahan P, Phan D, Stolte C, Daniel M-P (2007) Cognitive design principles for generating visualizations. In: Allen G (ed) Applied spatial cognition: from research to cognitive technology. Erlbaum, MahwahGoogle Scholar
- Tversky B, Corter JE, Yu L, Mason DL, Nickerson JV (2012) Representing category and continuum: visualizing thought. In: Rodgers P, Cox P, Plimmer B (eds) Diagrammatic representation and inference. Springer, Berlin, pp 23–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Vandenberg SG, Kuse AR (1978) Mental rotations, A group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual Motor Skills 47:599–604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zacks JM, Tversky B, Iyer G (2001) Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. J Exp Psychol: Gen 130:29–58Google Scholar