Cognitive Processing

, Volume 11, Issue 1, pp 9–20 | Cite as

The ‘I’ and the ‘Me’ in self-referential awareness: a neurocognitive hypothesis

Review

Abstract

The nature of the ‘self’ and self-referential awareness has been one of the most debated issues in philosophy, psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Understanding the neurocognitive bases of self-related representation and processing is also crucial to research on the neural correlates of consciousness. The distinction between an ‘I’, corresponding to a subjective sense of the self as a thinker and causal agent, and a ‘Me’, as the objective sense of the self with the unique and identifiable features constituting one’s self-image or self-concept, suggested by William James, has been re-elaborated by authors from different theoretical perspectives. In this article, empirical studies and theories about the ‘I’ and the ‘Me’ in cognition and self-related awareness are reviewed, including the relationships between self and perception, self and memory, the development of the self, self-referential stimulus processing, as well as related neuroimaging studies. Subsequently, the relations between self and different aspects of consciousness are considered. On the basis of the reviewed literature and with reference to Block’s distinction between phenomenal and access consciousness, a neurocognitive hypothesis is formulated about ‘I’-related and ‘Me’-related self-referential awareness. This hypothesis is extended to metacognitive awareness and a form of non-transitive consciousness, characteristic of meditation experiences and studies, with particular reference to the notion of mindfulness and other Buddhist constructs.

Keywords

Self Awareness Phenomenal consciousness Self-referential processing Mindfulness 

References

  1. Amsterdam BK (1972) Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Dev Psychobiol 5:297–305PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arenander A, Travis FT (2004) Brain patterns of self-awareness. In: Beitman B, Nair J (eds) Self-awareness deficits. W.W. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Baars BJ (1983) Conscious contents provide the nervous system with coherent, global information. In: Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE, Shapiro D (eds) Consciousness & self- regulation. Plenum Press, New York (p. 41)Google Scholar
  4. Baars BJ (1997) In the theatre of consciousness: global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 4:292–309Google Scholar
  5. Baars B (1998) Metaphors of consciousness and attention in the brain. Trends Neurosci 21:58–62PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baars BJ (2002) The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends Cogn Sci 6:47–52PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baars BJ, Ramsoy TZ, Laureys S (2003) Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends Neurosci 26:671–675PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barresi J (2002) From the thought is the thinker to the voice is the speaker. Theory Psychol 21:237–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bertenthal BI, Fischer KW (1978) The development of self recognition in the infant. Dev Psychol 14:44–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Block N (1996) How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends Neurosci 19:456–459PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Block N (2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behav Brain Sci 30:481–548PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyer P, Robbins P, Jack AI (2005) Varieties of self-systems worth having. Conscious Cogn 14:647–660PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brewer WF (1986) What is autobiographical memory? In: Rubin D (ed) Autobiographical memory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 25–49Google Scholar
  14. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11:49–57PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science 11:49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Butterworth GE (1992) Origins of self-perception in infancy. Psychol Inq 3:103–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cahn BRJ, Polich J (2006) Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull 132:180–211PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Clark A (1999) An embodied cognitive science? Trends Cogn Sci 3:345–351PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW (2000) The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev 107:261–288PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Courage ML, Howe ML (2002) From infant to child: the dynamics of cognitive change in the second year of life. Psychol Bull 128:250–277PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Craig AD (2004) Human feelings: why are some more aware than others? Trends Cogn Sci 8:239–241PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ (2004) Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci 7:189–195PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. d’Aquili E, Newberg A (1993) Religious and mystical states: a neuropsychological model. Zygon 28:177–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. d’Aquili E, Newberg A (1998) The neuropsychological basis of religion, or why God won’t go away. Zygon 33:187–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. d’Aquili E, Newberg A (2000) The neuropsychology of aesthetic, spiritual, and mystical states. Zygon 35:39–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Dalai Lama (1994) Transcendent wisdom: a teaching on the wisdom section of shantideva’s guide to the bodhisattva way of life, trans., ed., and annot. B. AlanWallace (Ithaca: Snow Lion)Google Scholar
  27. Damasio A (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York, HarcourtGoogle Scholar
  28. Duncan J (2001) An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:820–829PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Duncan J, Miller EK (2002) Cognitive focusing through adaptive neural coding in the primate prefrontal cortex. In: Stuss D, Knight RT (eds) Principles of frontal lobe function. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 278–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Edelman GM, Tononi G (2000) Universe of consciousness: how matter becomes imagination. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  31. Esslen M, Metzler S, Pascual-Marqui R, Jancke L (2008) Pre-reflective and reflective self-reference: a spatiotemporal EEG analysis. Neuroimage 42:437–449PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Farb NAS, Segal ZV, Mayberg H, Bean J, McKeon D, Zainab F, Anderson AK (2007) Attending to the present: meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2:313–322PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fossati P, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ, Grady C, Keightley ML, Craik F et al (2003) In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am J Psychiatry 160:1938–1945PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Freud S (1899) Screen memories, 3rd edn. The Hogarth Press, London, pp 301–322Google Scholar
  35. Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4:14–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends Cogn Sci 7:77–83PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gallese V (2003) The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathol 36:171–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gazzaniga MS (1985) The social brain. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  39. Gazzaniga MS (1998) The split brain revisited. Sci Am 279:50–55PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gillihan SJ, Farah MJ (2005) Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Psychol Bull 131:76–97PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Goldberg II, Harel M, Malach R (2006) When the brain loses its self: prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron 50:329–339PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Gopnik A, Meltzoff AN (1997) Words thoughts, and theories. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  43. Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98: 4259–4264Google Scholar
  44. Henri V, Henri C (1898) Earliest recollections. Popul Sci Mon 53:108–115Google Scholar
  45. Herzog H, Lele VR, Kuwert T et al (1990) Changed pattern of regional glucose metabolism during Yoga meditative relaxation. Neuropsychobiology 23:182–187 (1991)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Howe ML, Courage ML (1997) The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychol Rev 104:499–523PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Howe ML, Courage ML, Edison SC (2003) When autobiographical memory begins. In: Conway M, Gathercole S, Algarabel S, Pitarque A, Bajo T (eds) Theories of memory, vol III. Psychology Press, HoveGoogle Scholar
  48. James W (1890) Principles of psychology. Henry-Holt and Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  49. James W (1892) Psychology. Henry Holt and Company, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Johnson MK, Raye CL, Mitchell KJ, Touryan SR, Greene EJ, Nolen-Hoeksema S (2006) Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 1:56–64PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Johnstone B, Glass BA (2008) Support for a neuropsychological model of spirituality in persons with traumatic brain injury. Zygon 43:861–874CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kabat-Zinn J (2003) Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin Psychol: Sci Prac 10:144–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Kircher T, David AS (2003) The self and schizophrenia. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  54. Kjaer TW, Nowak M et al (2002) Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. NeuroImage 17:1080–1086PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET (2004) The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol 72:341–372PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Lamme V (2003) Why attention and awareness are different. Trends Cogn Sci 7:12–18PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Le Van Quien M (2003) Disentangling the dynamic core: a research program for a neurodynamics at the large-scale. Biol Res 36:67–88Google Scholar
  58. LeDoux J (1996) Emotional networks and motor control: a fearful view. Prog Brain Res 107:437–446PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. LeDoux J (2002) Synaptic self: how our brain become who we are. Viking Penguin, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  60. Legerstee M, Anderson D, Schaffer M (1998) Five and eight month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. Child Dev 69:37–50PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Legrand D (2003) How not to find the neural signature of self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 12:544–546PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Legrand D (2005) Being a body. Trends Cogn Sci 9:413–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Libby LK, Eibach RP (2002) Looking back in time: self-concept change and visual perspective in autobiographical memory. J Pers Soc Psychol 82:167–179PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM, Satpute AB (2004) Evidence-based and intuition-based self-knowledge: an fMRI study. J Pers Soc Psychol 87:421–435PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Lutz A, Thompson E (2003) Neurophenomenology: integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 10:31–52Google Scholar
  66. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ (2008) Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn Sci 12:163–169PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Mahesh Yogi M (1963) The science of being and art of living. Penguin Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  68. Maia TV, Cleeremans A (2005) Consciousness: converging insights from connectionist modeling and neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci 9:397–404PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Mead G (1934) Mind, self and society. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  70. Milner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Neisser U (1988) Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philos Psychol 1:35–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Nigro G, Neisser U (1983) Point of view in personal memories. Cogn Psychol 15:467–482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Northoff G (2004) Philosophy of the brain. The brain problem. John Benjamins Publishing, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  74. Northoff G, Bermpohl F (2004) Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci 8:102–107PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage 31:440–457PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Ochsner KN, Beer JS, Robertson ER, Cooper JC, Gabrieli JD, Kihlstrom JF et al (2005) The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage 28:797–814PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Panksepp J (1998) The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self. J Conscious Stud 5:566–582Google Scholar
  78. Panksepp J (2005) On the embodied neural nature of core emotional affects. J Conscious Stud 8–10:158–184Google Scholar
  79. Piaget J (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Povinelli DJ, Simon BB (1998) Young children’s understanding of briefly versus extremely delayed images of the self: emergence of the autobiographical stance. Dev Psychol 34:188–194PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Povinelli DJ, Landau KR, Perilloux HK (1996) Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Dev 67:1540–1554PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Raffone A, Srinivasan N (in press) An adaptive workspace hypothesis about the neural correlates of consciousness: insights from neuroscience and meditation studies. In Srinivasan N (ed) Progress in brain research: attention, vol 176. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 161–180Google Scholar
  83. Robinson JA (1986) Autobiographical memory: a historical prologue. In: Rubin DC (ed) Autobiographical memory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 19–24Google Scholar
  84. Rubin DC (ed) (1986) Autobiographical memory. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  85. Rubin DC (ed) (1996) Remembering our past. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  86. Rubin DC, Greenberg DL (1998) Visual memory deficit amnesia: a distinct amnesic presentation and etiology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95: 5413–5416Google Scholar
  87. Schacter DL, Addis DR (2007) The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos Trans R Soc (B) 362:773–786CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Schmitz TW, Kawahara-Baccus TN et al (2004) Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage 22:941–947PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Schore AN (2003) Affect regulation and the repair of the self. W.W. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  90. Shantideva (1997) A Guide to the bodhisattva way of life, trans. Vesna A. Wallace and B. Alan Wallace (Ithaca: Snow Lion)Google Scholar
  91. Siegel DJ (2007) The mindful brain. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  92. Sperling G (1960) The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychology Monographs 74, Whole no. 498Google Scholar
  93. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2008) Episodic memory and mental time travel. In: Dere E, Huston JP, Easton A (eds) Handbook of episodic memory research, vol 18. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 31–42Google Scholar
  94. Sumedho Ajahn (2004) Intuitive awareness. Amaravati Buddhist Monastery, Hemel Hempstead (UK)Google Scholar
  95. Suzuki S (1970) Zen mind, beginner’s mind. Weatherhill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  96. Thompson E, Varela F (2001) Radical embodiment. Trends Cogn Sci 5:418–425PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Tononi G, Edelman GM (1998) Consciousness and complexity. Science 282:1846–1851PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Travis F, Wallace RK (1999) Autonomic and EEG patterns during eyes-closed rest and transcendental meditation (TM) practice: the basis for a neural model of TM practice. Conscious Cogn 8:302–318PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W (eds) Organization of memory. Academic Press, New York, pp 381–403Google Scholar
  100. Tulving E (1983) Elements of episodic memory. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  101. Tulving E (2002) Chronesthesia: awareness of subjective time. In: Stuss DT, Knight RC (eds) Principles of frontal lobe functions. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 311–325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Varela FJ (1996) Neurophenomenology, a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J Conscious Stud 3:330–349Google Scholar
  103. Vasubandhu (1991) Abhidharma Kosabhasyam, French trans. Louis de La Vallée Poussin, English trans. Leo M. Pruden (Berkeley, CA: Asian Humanities Press)Google Scholar
  104. Vogeley K, Fink GR (2003) Neural correlates of first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 7:38–42PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. von Rospatt A (1995) The Buddhist doctrine of momentariness: a survey of the origins and early phase of this doctrine up to Vasubandhu Alt- und Neu-Indische Studien. Franz Steiner Verlag, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  106. Wallace BA (1999) The Buddhist tradition of samatha: methods for refining and examining consciousness. J Conscious Stud 6:175–187Google Scholar
  107. Williams M, Teasdale J, Segal Z, Kabat-Zinn J (2007) The mindful way through depression. Guilford Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  108. Zahavi D (2003) Phenomenology of self. In: Kircher T, David A (eds) The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  109. Zeki S (2003) The disunity of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 7:214–218PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Zelazo P, Frye D (1998) Cognitive complexity and control: II. The development of executive function in childhood. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 7:121–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Milan-BicoccaMilanItaly
  2. 2.Department of Psychology‘Sapienza’ University of RomeRomeItaly
  3. 3.Perceptual Dynamics LaboratoryBrain Science Institute RIKENSaitamaJapan

Personalised recommendations