Cognitive Processing

, Volume 10, Issue 2, pp 105–111

Brain and mind operational architectonics and man-made “machine” consciousness

  • Andrew A. Fingelkurts
  • Alexander A. Fingelkurts
  • Carlos F. H. Neves
Review
  • 253 Downloads

Abstract

To build a true conscious robot requires that a robot’s “brain” be capable of supporting the phenomenal consciousness as human’s brain enjoys. Operational Architectonics framework through exploration of the temporal structure of information flow and inter-area interactions within the network of functional neuronal populations [by examining topographic sharp transition processes in the scalp electroencephalogram (EEG) on the millisecond scale] reveals and describes the EEG architecture which is analogous to the architecture of the phenomenal world. This suggests that the task of creating the “machine” consciousness would require a machine implementation that can support the kind of hierarchical architecture found in EEG.

Keywords

EEG Neural assemblies Artificial intelligence Robot mind Cognition Phenomena Operation Synchrony Metastability Subjective experience Embodied approach Virtual reality Synthetic 

References

  1. Aleksander I, Dunmall B (2003) Axioms and tests for the presence of minimal consciousness in agents. J Conscious Stud 10:7–18Google Scholar
  2. Allen JA (1994) Delineating conscious and unconscious processes: commentary on Baars on contrastive analysis. Psyche 1(9). http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.an/v2/psyche-1-9-allen.html
  3. Block N (1995) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 18:227–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bressler SL, Kelso JAS (2001) Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 5:26–36. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01564-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chalmers DJ (1995) Facing up to the problems of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 2:200–219Google Scholar
  6. Chalmers DJ (1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  7. Chella A, Manzotti R (2007) Artificial consciousness. Imprint Academic, ExeterGoogle Scholar
  8. Chrisley R, Parthemore J (2007) Synthetic phenomenology: exploiting embodiment to specify the non-conceptual content of visual experience. J Conscious Stud 14:44–58Google Scholar
  9. Clowes R, Torrance S, Chrisley R (2007) Machine consciousness: embodiment and imagination. J Conscious Stud 14:7–14Google Scholar
  10. Dainton B (2000) Stream of consciousness. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness explained. Little Brown, BostonGoogle Scholar
  12. Dreyfus HL (2007) Why Heideggerian AI failed and how fixing it would require making it more Heideggerian. Philos Psychol 20:247–268. doi:10.1080/09515080701239510 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2001) Operational architectonics of the human brain biopotential field: towards solving the mind-brain problem. Brain Mind 2:261–296. doi:10.1023/A:1014427822738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2003) Operational architectonics of perception and cognition: A principle of self-organized metastable brain states, VI Parmenides Workshop “Perception and Thinking” of the Institute of Medical Psychology. University of Munich, Elba 5–10 AprilGoogle Scholar
  15. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2004a) The Operational Architectonics concept of brain and mind functioning. Congress on Modeling Mental Processes and Disorders. Kusadasi 24–29 MayGoogle Scholar
  16. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2004b) Making complexity simpler: multivariability and metastability in the brain. Int J Neurosci 114:843–862. doi:10.1080/00207450490450046 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2005) Mapping of the brain operational architectonics. In: Chen FJ (ed) Focus on brain mapping research, Chap 2. Nova Science Publishers Inc, New York, pp 59–98Google Scholar
  18. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2006) Timing in cognition and EEG brain dynamics: discreteness versus continuity. Cogn Process 7:135–162. doi:10.1007/s10339-006-0035-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2008) Brain-mind Operational Architectonics imaging: technical and methodological aspects. Open Neuroimag J 2:73–93CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Freeman WJ (2007a) Indirect biological measures of consciousness from field studies of brains as dynamical systems. Neural Netw 20:1021–1031. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.004 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Freeman WJ (2007b) Definitions of state variables and state space for brain-computer interface. Part 1. Multiple hierarchical levels of brain function. Cogn Neurodyn 1:3–14. doi:10.1007/s11571-006-9001-x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gamez D (2005) An ordinal probability scale for synthetic phenomenology. In: Chrisley R, Clowes R, Torrance S (eds) Proceedings of the AISB05 symposium on next generation approaches to machine consciousness. Hatfield, UK, pp 85–94Google Scholar
  23. Gamez D (2006) The XML approach to synthetic phenomenology. In: Chrisley R, Clowes R, Torrance S (eds) Proceedings of the AISB06 symposium on integrative approaches to machine consciousness. Bristol, UK, pp 128–135Google Scholar
  24. Gamez D (2008) Progress in machine consciousness. Conscious Cogn 17:887–910. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.005 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Griffin DR (1998) Unsnarling the world-knot: consciousness, freedom and the mind-body problem. University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  26. Haikonen PO (2007a) Robot brains: circuits and systems for conscious machines. Wiley, UKGoogle Scholar
  27. Haikonen PO (2007b) Essential issues of conscious machines. J Conscious Stud 14:72–84Google Scholar
  28. Hesslow G, Jirenhed D-A (2007) The inner world of a simple robot. J Conscious Stud 14:85–96Google Scholar
  29. Holland O (2003) Editorial introduction. J Conscious Stud 12:1–6Google Scholar
  30. Ikegami T (2007) Simulating active perception and mental imagery with embodied chaotic itinerancy. J Conscious Stud 14:111–125Google Scholar
  31. James W (1890) The principles of psychology, vol I. Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Kaplan AYa, Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA, Borisov SV, Darkhovsky BS (2005) Nonstationary nature of the brain activity as revealed by EEG/MEG: methodological, practical and conceptual challenges. Signal Process 85:2190–2212. doi:10.1016/j.sigpro.2005.07.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  34. Kiverstein J (2007) Could a robot have a subjective point of view? J Conscious Stud 14:127–139Google Scholar
  35. Koch C (2004) The quest for consciousness: a neurobiological approach. Roberts & Company Publishers, Englewood (Col)Google Scholar
  36. Koch C, Tononi G (2008) Can machines be conscious? IEEE Spectr 6:47–51Google Scholar
  37. Laureys S, Pellas F, Van Eeckhout F, Ghorbel S, Schnakers C et al (2005) The locked-in syndrome: what is it like to be conscious but paralyzed and voiceless? In: Laureys S (ed) Progress in brain research, vol 150. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 495–511Google Scholar
  38. Lehar S (2003) Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of subjective conscious experience: a gestalt bubble model. Behav Brain Sci 26:375–408PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. von der Malsburg C (1999) The what and why of binding: the modeler’s perspective. Neuron 24:95–104. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80825-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Manzotti R (2006) A process oriented view of conscious perception. J Conscious Stud 13:7–41Google Scholar
  41. Manzotti R (2007) Towards artificial consciousness. APA Newsl Philos Comput 07:12–15Google Scholar
  42. Markram H (2006) The blue brain project. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:153–160. doi:10.1038/nrn1848 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. McFadden J (2002) Synchronous firing and its influence on the brain’s electromagnetic field. Evidence for an electromagnetic field theory of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 9:23–50Google Scholar
  44. Metzinger T (2003) Being no one. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  45. Moran D (2000) Introduction to phenomenology. Routledge, London Google Scholar
  46. Muller GE (1896) Zur psychophysik der gesichtsempfindungen. Z Psychol 10:1–82Google Scholar
  47. Nagel T (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Phyl Rev 83:435–450. doi:10.2307/2183914 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nunez PL (2000) Toward a quantitative description of large-scale neocortical dynamic function and EEG. Behav Brain Sci 23:371–398. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003253 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pockett S (2000) The nature of consciousness: a hypothesis. Writers Club Press, LincolnGoogle Scholar
  50. Revonsuo A (2001) Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? J Conscious Stud 8:3–23Google Scholar
  51. Revonsuo A (2006) Inner presence: Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  52. Searle JR (1992) The rediscovery of the mind. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  53. Shannon CE (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Sys Tech J 27(379–423):623–656Google Scholar
  54. Shapiro LA (2000) Multiple realizations. J Philos 97:635–654. doi:10.2307/2678460 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sloman A, Chrisley RL (2005) More things than are dreamt of in your biology: Information-processing in biologically inspired robots. Cogn Syst Res 6:145–174. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2004.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Stening J, Jacobsson H, Ziemke T (2005) Imagination and abstraction of sensorimotor flow: Towards a robot model. In: Chrisley R, Clowes R, Torrance S (eds) Proceedings of the AISB05 symposium on next generation approaches to machine consciousness. Hatfield, UK, pp 50–58Google Scholar
  57. Stubenberg L (1998) Consciousness and qualia. John Benjamins, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  58. Warfield JN (1977) Crossing theory and hierarchy mapping. IEEE Trans Syst Man Cybern 7:505–523. doi:10.1109/TSMC.1977.4309760 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Whitehead AN (1929/1978) Process and Reality, Free Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  60. Zeki S (2003) The disunity of consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 7:214–218. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00081-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Zeki S, Bartels A (1999) Towards a theory of visual consciousness. Consious Cogn 8:225–259. doi:10.1006/ccog.1999.0390 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Ziemke T (2007) What’s life got to do with it? In: Chella A, Manzotti R (eds) Artificial consciousness. Imprint Academic, Exeter , pp 48–66Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Andrew A. Fingelkurts
    • 1
  • Alexander A. Fingelkurts
    • 1
  • Carlos F. H. Neves
    • 1
  1. 1.BM-Science - Brain and Mind Technologies Research CentreEspooFinland

Personalised recommendations