Cognitive Processing

, Volume 9, Issue 2, pp 107–119 | Cite as

Spatial term apprehension with a reference object’s rotation in three-dimensional space

Research Report

Abstract

It has been reported that the overall shapes of spatial categorical patterns of projective spatial terms such as above and below are not influenced by the rotation of a reference object on a two-dimensional (2D) upright plane. However, is this also true in three-dimensional (3D) space? This study shows the dynamic aspects of the apprehension of projective spatial terms in 3D space by detailing how the rotation of a reference object with an inherent front influences the apprehension of projective spatial terms on a level plane by mapping their spatial categorical patterns. The experiment was designed to examine how spatial categorical patterns on a level plane changed with the rotation of a reference object with an inherent front in 3D computer graphics space. We manipulated the rotation of a reference object with an inherent front at three levels (0°, 90°, and 180° rotations) and examined how such manipulation changed the overall spatial categorical patterns of four basic Japanese projective spatial terms: mae, ushiro, hidari, and migi (similar to in front of, behind, to the left of, and to the right of in English, respectively). The results show that spatial term apprehension was affected by the rotation of the reference object in 3D space. In particular, rotation influenced the mae–ushiro and hidari–migi systems differently. The results also imply that our understanding of projective spatial terms on a level plane in 3D space is affected dynamically by visual information from 3D cues.

Keywords

Spatial cognition Spatial term apprehension Rotation of a reference object Three-dimensional space 

References

  1. Burigo M, Coventry KR (2004) Spatial language and reference frame assignment: the role of the located object. P Cogn Sci Soc 168–173Google Scholar
  2. Burigo M, Coventry KR (2005) Spatial language, reference frames and alternative models. P Cogn Sci Soc 2454Google Scholar
  3. Carlson LA, Kenny R (2005) Constraints on spatial language comprehension: function and geometry. In: Pecher D, Zwaan RA (eds) Grounding cognition: the role of perception and action in memory, language and thinking. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 35–64Google Scholar
  4. Carlson LA, Logan GD (2001) Using spatial terms to select an object. Mem Cogn 29:883–892Google Scholar
  5. Carlson LA, West R, Taylor HA (2002) Neural correlates of spatial term use. J Exp Psychol Human 28:1391–1408Google Scholar
  6. Carlson-Radvansky LA, Logan GD (1997) The influence of reference frame selection on spatial template construction. J Mem Lang 37:411–437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carlson-Radvansky LA, Tang Z (2000) Functional influences on orienting a reference frame. Mem Cogn 28:812–820Google Scholar
  8. Carlson-Radvansky LA, Covey ES, Lattanzi KM (1999) “What” effects on “where”: functional influences on spatial relations. Psychol Sci 10:516–521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Coventry KR, Garrod SC (2004) Saying, seeing and acting: the psychological semantics of spatial prepositions. Psychology press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  10. Coventry KR, Prat-Sala M, Richards L (2001) The interplay between geometry and function in the comprehension of over, under, above, and below. J Mem Lang 44:376–398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Coventry KR, Cangelosi A, Newstead S, Bacon A, Rajapakse R (2005) Grounding natural language quantifiers in visual attention. P Cogn Sci Soc 506–511Google Scholar
  12. Hayward WG, Tarr MJ (1995) Spatial language and spatial representation. Cognition 55:39–84PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Herskovits A (1986) Language and spatial cognition: an interdisciplinary study of the prepositions of English. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Imai M, Nakanishi T, Miyashita H, Kidachi Y, Ishizaki S (1999) The meanings of front/back/left/right. Cogn Stud 6:207–225Google Scholar
  15. Kojima T, Kusumi T (2006) The effect of the extra object on the linguistic apprehension of spatial relationship between two objects. Spat Cogn Comput 6:145–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kojima T, Kusumi T (2007) Computing positions indicated by spatial terms in three-dimensional space. Psychologia 50:205–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Landau B, Jackendoff R (1993) “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behav Brain Sci 16:217–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lawson R, Humphreys GW, Jolicoeur P (2000) The combined effects of plane disorientation and foreshortening on picture naming: one manipulation or two? J Exp Psychol Human 26:568–581Google Scholar
  19. Leek EC (1998) Effects of stimulus orientation on the identification of common polyoriented objects. Psychon Bull Rev 5:650–658Google Scholar
  20. Levinson SC (1996) Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: crosslinguistic evidence. In: Bloom P, Peterson MA, Nadel L, Garrett M (eds) Language and space. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 109–169Google Scholar
  21. Logan GD, Sadler DD (1996) A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations. In: Bloom P, Peterson MA, Nadel L, Garrett M (eds) Language and space. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 493–529Google Scholar
  22. Miller GA, Johnson-Laird PN (1976) Language and perception. Belknap Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  23. Munnich E, Landau B, Dosher BA (2001) Spatial language and spatial representation: a cross-linguistic comparison. Cognition 81:171–207PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Norman JF, Todd JT, Perotti VJ, Tittle JS (1996) The visual perception of three-dimensional length. J Exp Psychol Human 22:173–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Norman JF, Lappin JS, Norman HF (2000) The perception length on curved and flat surfaces. Percept Psychophys 62:1133–1145PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Norman JF, Norman HF, Lee YM, Stockton D (2004) The visual perception of length along intrinsically curved surfaces. Percept Psychophys 66:77–88PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Cognitive Psychology in Education, Graduate School of EducationKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations