Advertisement

Cognitive Processing

, Volume 8, Issue 2, pp 79–91 | Cite as

Cognition as coordinated non-cognition

  • Lawrence W. BarsalouEmail author
  • Cynthia Breazeal
  • Linda B. Smith
Review

Abstract

We propose that cognition is more than a collection of independent processes operating in a modular cognitive system. Instead, we propose that cognition emerges from dependencies between all of the basic systems in the brain, including goal management, perception, action, memory, reward, affect, and learning. Furthermore, human cognition reflects its social evolution and context, as well as contributions from a developmental process. After presenting these themes, we illustrate their application to the process of anticipation. Specifically, we propose that anticipations occur extensively across domains (i.e., goal management, perception, action, reward, affect, and learning) in coordinated manners. We also propose that anticipation is central to situated action and to social interaction, and that many of its key features reflect the process of development.

Keywords

Coordination Development Embodiment Robotics Situated cognition Social interaction 

Notes

Acknowledgment

DARPA contract BICA FA8650-05-C-7255 awarded to three authors supported their collaboration on this project. In addition, Lawrence Barsalou was supported by DARPA contract FA8650-05-C-7256 and National Science Foundation Grant BCS-0212134. We are grateful to Arthur Glenberg and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on a draft of this article.

References

  1. Ashby FG, Ell SW, Valentin VV, Casale MB (2005) FROST: a distributed neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance. J Cogn Neurosci 17:1728–1743PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett LF (2006) Emotions as natural kinds? Perspect Psychol Sci 1:28–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2007) The experience of emotion. Annu Rev Psychol 58:373–413PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barsalou LW (1987) The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In: Neisser U (ed) Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 101–140Google Scholar
  5. Barsalou LW (1989) Intraconcept similarity and its implications for interconcept similarity. In: Vosniadou S, Ortony A (eds) Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 76–121Google Scholar
  6. Barsalou LW (1993) Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In: Collins AC, Gathercole SE, Conway MA (eds) Theories of memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London, pp 29–101Google Scholar
  7. Barsalou LW (1999a) Language comprehension: archival memory or preparation for situated action. Discourse Process 28:61–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barsalou LW (1999b) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Barsalou LW (2003) Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Lang Cogn Process 18:513–562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Barsalou LW (2005) Continuity of the conceptual system across species. Trends Cogn Sci 9:309–311PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Barsalou LW (in press). Embodied cognition. Annu Rev PsycholGoogle Scholar
  12. Barsalou LW (in press-b). Situating concepts. In: Robbins P, Aydede M (eds) Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Barsalou LW, Niedenthal PM, Barbey A, Ruppert J (2003) Social embodiment. In: Ross B (eds) The psychology of learning and motivation. vol 43. Academic, San Diego, pp 43–92Google Scholar
  14. Bjorklund DF, Pellegrini AD (2000) Child development and evolutionary psychology. Child Dev 71:1687–1708PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Blakemore SJ, Decety J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:561–567PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bosbach S, Cole J, Prinz W, Knoblich G (2005) Inferring another’s expectation from action: the role of peripheral sensation. Nat Neurosci 8:1295–1297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Breazeal C (2003) Emotion and sociable humanoid robots. In: Hudlika E (ed) International journal of human computer interaction 59:119–155Google Scholar
  18. Breazeal C, Aryananda L (2002) Recognizing affective intent in robot directed speech. Auton Robots 12:85–104Google Scholar
  19. Breazeal C, Buchsbaum D, Gray J, Gatenby D, Blumberg B (2005) Learning from and about others: towards using imitation to bootstrap the social understanding of others by robots. Artif Life 11:1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Brooks RA (1991) Intelligence without representation. Artif Intell 47:139–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Chandrasekaran B, Josephson JR (2000) Function in device representation. Eng Comput 16:162–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Campos J, Stenberg C (1981) Perception, appraisal, and emotion: the onset of social referencing. In: Lamb M, Sherrod L (eds) Infant social cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 273–314Google Scholar
  23. Chao LL, Martin A (2000) Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage 12:478–484PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Clark A (1997) Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  25. Damasio AR (1989) Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 33:25–62PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error. Grosset/Putnam, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  27. Decety J, Grèzes J (2006) The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res 1079:4–14PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Delcomyn F (1980) Neural basis of rhythmic behavior in animals. Science 210:492–498PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Donald M (1993) Precis of “Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.” Behav Brain Sci 16:739–791Google Scholar
  30. Feinman S (1982) Social referencing in infancy. Merrill Palmer Q 28:445–470Google Scholar
  31. Fernald A (1989) Intonation and communicative intent in mother’s speech to infants: is the melody the message? Child Dev 60:1497–1510PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Forbus K (1993) Qualitative process theory: twelve years after. Artif Intell 59:115–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8:396–403PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gentner D, Stevens AL (eds) (1983) Mental models. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, MahwahGoogle Scholar
  35. Glenberg AM (1997) What memory is for. Behav Brain Sci 20:1–55PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Glenberg AM, Kaschak MP (2002) Grounding language in action. Psychon Bull Rev 9:558–569PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Granger R (2006) Engines of the brain: the computational instruction set of human cognition. AI Mag 27:15–32Google Scholar
  38. Gray J, Breazeal C, Berlin M, Brooks A, Lieberman J (2005) Action parsing and goal inference using self as simulator. In: Bowerman M, Levinson S (eds) Language acquisition and conceptual development. In: Proceedings of fourteenth IEEE workshop on robot and human interactive communication (Ro-Man05). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 111–128Google Scholar
  39. Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F (2004) Somatopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41:301–307PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Havas DA, Glenberg AM, Rinck M (in press) Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychon Bull RevGoogle Scholar
  41. Hayes PJ (1985) The second naive physics manifesto. In: Hobbs JR, Moore RC (eds) Formal theories of the commonsense world. Ablex, NorwoodGoogle Scholar
  42. Hutchins E (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  43. Iacoboni M (in press) Understanding others: Imitation, language, empathy. In: Hurley S, Chater N (eds) Perspectives on imitation: from cognitive neuroscience to social science. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  44. Izard C (1977) Human emotions. Plenum, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. James W (1890) The principles of psychology. Dove, MineolaGoogle Scholar
  46. Jones S (2006) Infants learn to imitate by being imitated. In: Fifth proceedings of the international conference of development and learningGoogle Scholar
  47. Jones SS, Smith LB (1993) The place of perception in children’s concepts. Cogn Dev 8:113–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kintsch W, van Dijk TA (1978) Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychol Rev 85:363–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Martin A (2001) Functional neuroimaging of semantic memory. In: Cabeza R, Kingstone A (eds) Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge pp 153–186Google Scholar
  50. Martin A (2007) The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annu Rev Psychol 58:25–45PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE, the PDP Research Group (1986) Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. vol 2. Psychological and biological models. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  52. Meltzoff A, Moore MK (1997) Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev Parent 6:179–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Meltzoff AN (1996) The human infant as imitative generalist: a 20 year progress report on infant imitation with implications for comparative psychology. In: Heyes CM, Galef BG (eds) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  54. Newell A, Simon HA (1972) Human problem solving. Prentice-Hall, Englewood CliffsGoogle Scholar
  55. Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P, Krauth-Gruber S, Ric F (2005) Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 9:184–211PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. O’Regan JK, Noe A (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 24:939–1011PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Piaget J (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  58. Plutchik R (1991) The emotions. University Press of America, LanhamGoogle Scholar
  59. Port R, van Gelder T (eds) (1995) Mind as motion: explorations in the dynamics of cognition. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  60. Repp BH, Knoblich G (2004) Perceiving action identity: how pianists recognize their own performances. Psychol Sci 15:604–609PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rips LJ (1994) The psychology of proof: deduction in human thinking. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  62. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2002) From mirror neurons to imitation: facts and speculations. In: Meltzoff AN, Prinz W (eds) The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 247–266Google Scholar
  63. Robbins P, Aydede M (eds) (in press) Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  64. Rovee-Collier C, Hayne H (1987) Reaction of infant memory: implications for cognitive development. In: Reese H (ed) Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol 2. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  65. Rumelhart DE, McClelland JL, The PDP Research Group (1986) Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. vol 1. Foundations. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  66. Samuelson LK, Smith LB (2000) Children’s attention to rigid and deformable shape in naming and non-naming tasks. Child dev 71:1555–1570PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Schank RC, Abelson RP (1977) Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  68. Simmons WK, Martin A, Barsalou LW (2005) Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cereb Cortex 15:1602–1608PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Smith LB, Gasser M (2005) The development of embodied cognition: Six lessons from babies. Artif Life 11:13–30PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Smith ER, Semin GR (2004) Socially situated cognition: cognition in its social context. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 36:53–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Smith LB, Thelen E (1993) (eds) A dynamical systems approach to development: applications. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  72. Spivey M (2006) The continuity of mind. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  73. Strack F, Martin LL, Stepper S (1988) Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:768–777PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Thelen E, Smith LB (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and actions. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  75. Thomaz AL, Berlin M, Breazeal C (2005) An embodied computational model of social referencing. In: Proceedings of fourteenth IEEE workshop on robot and human interactive communication (Ro-Man05)Google Scholar
  76. Tomasello M, Kruger A, Ratner H (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Van Orden GC, Holden JG, Turvey MT (2003) Self-organization of cognitive performance. J Exp Psychol Gen 132:331–350PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Wilson M, Knoblich G (2005) The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspsecifics. Psychol Bull 131:460–473PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wolpert DM, Doya K, Kawato M (2003) A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Biol Sci 358:593–602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1999) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Zwaan RA, Stanfield RA, Yaxley RH (2002) Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychol Sci 13:168–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Zwaan RA, Taylor LJ (2006) Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. J Exp Psychol Gen 135:1–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lawrence W. Barsalou
    • 1
    Email author
  • Cynthia Breazeal
    • 2
  • Linda B. Smith
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyEmory UniversityAtlantaUSA
  2. 2.Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyCambridgeUSA
  3. 3.Indiana UniversityBloomingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations