Advertisement

Acoustic differentiation in a sub-oscine calls: females call with more entropy than males

  • Alejandro Ariel Ríos-ChelénEmail author
  • Xhareni Díaz-Lezama
  • Bibiana Montoya
Original Article

Abstract

We quantified variation at the individual, pair and sex levels in acoustic attributes of Vermilion Flycatcher (Pyrocephalus rubinus)’s peent calls. We recorded calls of 15 pairs during the breeding season, and analyzed seven acoustic measures encompassing temporal, frequency and entropy measures. We run Principal Component Analysis to reduce the number of acoustic parameters; the first three principal components (PCs: PC1, PC2, and PC3) were related to call frequency, call entropy and call duration, respectively. These PCs were further used in Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA), and logistic regressions. Although all three PCs varied more among than within individuals, the DFA correctly classified only 57% of males and 63% of females, casting doubts on the possibility that the peent functions as an individual marker. At the pair level, only PC1 (call frequency) differed among pairs; however, the DFA correctly classified only 6.7%. of pairs. At the sex level, male peents had lower values of PC2 (call entropy) than female peents; the DFA correctly classified 66.7% and 73.3% of individuals as males and females, respectively (mean correct classification 70%). Binary logistic regression confirmed that only call entropy differed between sexes, and correctly classified 73.3% of females and 80% of males. Our data show sexual acoustic dimorphism in the peent, and open the possibility that variation in peent entropy may allow for sex discrimination in this species.

Keywords

Vermilion flycatcher Pyrocephalus rubinus Suboscine Call Sexual dimorphism Animal communication 

Zusammenfassung

Akustische Differenzierung bei suboszinen Rufen: Weibchen rufen mit mehr Entropie als Männchen. Wir quantifizierten die Variation akustischer Eigenschaften des sogenannten Peent-Rufes (ein spitzer und langer Ruf, engl. peent call) beim Rubintyrann (Pyrocephalus rubinus) auf individueller, paarbezogener und geschlechtsspezifischer Ebene. Dafür nahmen wir Rufe von 15 Paaren während der Brutsaison auf und analysierten sieben akustische Parameter, die Zeit-, Frequenz- und Entropiemessungen umfassten. Wir führten Hauptkomponentenanalysen durch, um die Anzahl an akustischen Parametern zu reduzieren. Die ersten drei Hauptkomponenten (engl. principal components, PCs: PC1, PC2 und PC3) standen im Zusammenhang mit Ruffrequenz, -entropie bzw. -dauer. Diese PCs wurden weiter für Diskriminanzanalysen (engl. Discriminant Function Analysis; DFA) und logistische Regressionen verwendet. Auch wenn alle drei PCs größere Variationen zwischen den Individuen als innerhalb der Individuen zeigten, klassifizierte die DFA nur 57% der Männchen und 63% der Weibchen korrekt. Dies lässt Zweifel an der Funktion des Peent-Rufes als individuelles Kennzeichen aufkommen. Auf paarbezogener Ebene zeigte nur PC1 (Ruffrequenz) Unterschiede zwischen den Arten. Jedoch klassifizierte die DFA nur 6.7% der Paare korrekt. Auf geschlechtsspezifischer Ebene zeigten männliche Peent-Rufe geringere Werte bei der PC2 (Rufentropie) als weibliche Peent-Rufe. Die DFA klassifizierte 66.7% bzw. 73.3% der Individuen korrekt als Männchen bzw. Weibchen (durchschnittlich richtige Klassifizierung 70%). Die binäre logistische Regression bestätigte, dass sich nur die Rufentropie zwischen den Geschlechtern unterschied und klassifizierte 73.3% der Weibchen und 80% der Männchen korrekt. Unsere Daten weisen auf einen akustischen Sexualdimorphismus beim Peent-Ruf hin und eröffnen die Möglichkeit, die Variation in der Peent-Entropie zur Geschlechtsunterscheidung in dieser Vogelart zu nutzen.

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Margarita Martínez-Gómez for logistic support. We are grateful to Laura Corkovic who translated the title and abstract from English to German. We are thankful to two reviewers who improved the quality of this paper. This study complies with the current laws of the country in which they were performed.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

10336_2019_1740_MOESM1_ESM.wav (196 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (WAV 196 kb)
10336_2019_1740_MOESM2_ESM.wav (154 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (WAV 153 kb)
10336_2019_1740_MOESM3_ESM.wav (159 kb)
Supplementary material 3 (WAV 159 kb)
10336_2019_1740_MOESM4_ESM.wav (118 kb)
Supplementary material 4 (WAV 117 kb)

References

  1. Aubin T, Mathevon N, Staszewski V, Boulinier T (2007) Acoustic communication in the Kittiwake Rissa tridactyla: potential cues for sexual and individual signatures in long calls. Polar Biol 30:1027–1033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bard SC, Hau M, Wikelski M, Wingfield JC (2002) Vocal distinctiveness and response to conspecific playback in the spotted antbird, a neotropical suboscine. Condor 104:387–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beecher MD, Beecher IM, Hahn S (1981) Parent-offspring recognition in bank swallows (Riparia riparia): II. Development and acoustic basis. Animal Behav 29:95–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beer CG (1971) Individual recognition of voice in the social behavior of birds. Adv Stud Behav 3:27–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berg KS, Delgado S, Okawa R, Beissinger SR, Bradbury JW (2011) Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus. Animal Behav 81:241–248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bourgeois K, Curé C, Legrand J, Gómez-Díaz E, Vidal E, Aubin T, Mathevon N (2007) Morphological versus acoustic analysis: what is the most efficient method for sexing yelkouan shearwaters Puffinus yelkouan? J Ornithol 148:261–269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bradbury JW, Cortopassi KA, Clemmons JR (2001) Geographical variation in the contact calls of orange-fronted parakeets. Auk 118:958–972CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Breitwisch R, Diaz M, Gottlieb N, Lee R, Zaias J (1986) Defense of fall territories by mated and unmated Northern mockingbirds in Southern Florida. J Field Ornithol 57:16–21Google Scholar
  9. Bretagnolle V, Lequette B (1990) Structural variation in the call of the Cory’s Shearwater (Calonectris diomedea, Aves, Procellariidae). Ethology 85:313–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown ED, Farabaugh SM (1990) Macrogeographic variation in alarm calls of the Australian magpie Gymnorhina tibicen. Bird Behav 9:64–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buhrman-Deever SC, Hobson EA, Hobson AD (2008) Individual recognition and selective response to contact calls in foraging brown-throated conures, Aratinga pertinax. Animal Behav 76:1715–1725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  13. Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (1995) Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P, Aubin T (2001) Acoustic communication in a black-headed gull colony: how chicks identify their parent? Ethology 107:961–974CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Clark JA, Leung J (2011) Vocal distinctiveness and information coding in a suboscine with multiple song types: Eastern Wood-Pewee. Wilson J Ornithol 123:835–840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Clay Z, Smith CL, Blumstein DT (2012) Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: what do these calls really mean? Animal Behav 83:323–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Courter JR, Ritchison G (2010) Alarm calls of tufted titmice convey information about predator size and threat. Behav Ecol 21:936–942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Curé C, Aubin T, Mathevon N (2011) Sex discrimination and mate recognition by voice in the yelkouan shearwater Puffinus yelkouan. Bioacoustics 20:235–250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Curé C, Mathevon N, Aubin T (2016) Mate vocal recognition in the Scopoli’s shearwater Calonectris diomedea: do females and males share the same acoustic code? Behav Process 128:96–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dahl JA, Ritchison G (2018) Responses of blue jays (Cyanocitta cristata) to raptors that differ in predatory threat. Avian Biol Research 11:159–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. de Brooke ML (1978) Sexual differences in the voice and individual vocal recognition in the Manx shearwater (Puffinus Puffinus). Animal Behav 26:622–629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dentressangle F, Aubin T, Mathevon N (2012) Males use time whereas females prefer harmony: individual call recognition in the dimorphic blue-footed booby. Animal Behav 84:413–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dhondt AA, Lambrechts MM (1992) Individual voice recognition in birds. TREE 7:178–179PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Dooling RJ, Park TJ, Brown SD, Okanoya K, Soli SD (1987) Perceptual organization of acoustical stimuli in the budgerigar: II. Vocal signals. J Comp Psychol 101:367–381PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Doutrelant C, Leitao A, Otter K, Lambrechts MM (2000) Effect of blue tit song syntax on great tit territorial responsiveness—an experimental test of the character shift hypothesis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:119–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eda-Fujiwara H, Kanesada A, Okamoto Y, Satoh R, Watanabe A, Miyamoto T (2011) Long-term maintenance and eventual extinction of preference for a mate’s call in the female budgerigar. Animal Behav 82:971–979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Falls JB (1982) Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, Cambridge, pp 237–278Google Scholar
  28. Farnsworth A (2005) Flight calls and their value for future ornithological studies and conservation research. Auk 122:733–746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Fitzsimmons LP, Barker NK, Mennill DJ (2008) Individual variation and lek-based vocal distinctiveness in songs of the screaming piha (Lipaugus vociferans), a suboscine songbird. Auk 125:908–914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69–73PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Griesser M (2009) Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species. Proc R Soc B 276:2887–2892PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Gyger M, Marler P (1988) Food calling in the domestic fowl, Callus gallus: the role of external referents and deception. Animal Behav 36:358–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hanna D, Blouin-Demers G, Wilson DR, Mennill DJ (2011) Anthropogenic noise affects song structure in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). J Exp Biol 214:3549–3556PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Hutchison RE, Stevenson JG, Thorpe WH (1968) The basis for individual recognition by voice in the sandwich tern (Sterna sandvicensis). Behaviour 32:150–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ippi S, Vázquez RA, Van Dongen WFD, Lazzoni I (2011) Geographical variation in the vocalizations of the suboscine Thorn-tailed Rayadito Aphrastura spinicauda. Ibis 153:789–805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. James PC, Robertson HA (1985) The calls of male and female Madeiran storm-petrels (Oceanodroma castro). Auk 102:391–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jouventin P, Aubin T, Lengagne T (1999) Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the acoustic system of individual recognition. Animal Behav 57:1175–1183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kipper S, Kiefer S, Bartsch C, Weiss M (2015) Female calling? Song responses to conspecific call playbacks in nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos. Animal Behav 100:60–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Koloff J, Mennill DJ (2013) Vocal behaviour of Barred Antshrikes, a Neotropical duetting suboscine bird. J Ornithol 154:51–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kondo N, Watanabe S (2009) Contact calls: information and social function. Jpn Psychol Res 51:197–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kondo N, Izawa E-I, Watanabe S (2012) Crows cross-modally recognize group members but not non-group members. Proc R Soc B 279:1937–1942PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Lein MR (2008) Song variation in Buff-breasted flycatchers (Empidonax fulvifrons). Wilson J Ornithol 20:256–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lind H, Dabelsteen T, McGregor PK (1996) Female great tits can identify mates by song. Animal Behav 52:667–671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lovell SF, Lein MR (2004a) Song variation in a population of alder flycatchers. J Field Ornithol 75:146–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lovell SF, Lein MR (2004b) Neighbor–stranger discrimination by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher, Empidonax alnorum. Behav Ecol 15:799–804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lovell SF, Lein MR (2005) Individual recognition of neighbors by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher Empidonax alnorum. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:623–630CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Marler P (2004) Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication. In: Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (eds) Nature´s music. The science of birdsong. Elsevier Academic Press, Cambridge, pp 132–177Google Scholar
  48. Miyasato LE, Baker MC (1999) Black-capped chickadee call dialects along a continuous habitat corridor. Animal Behav 57:1311–1318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mundiger PC (1970) Vocal imitation and individual recognition of finch calls. Science 168:480–482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Nowicki S (1983) Flock-specific recognition of chickadee calls. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:317–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Okanoya K, Kimura T (1993) Acoustical and perceptual structures of sexually dimorphic distance calls in bengalese finches (Lonchura striata domestica). J Comp Psychol 107:386–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Radford AN (2004) Voice breaking in males results in sexual dimorphism of green woodhoopoe calls. Behaviour 141:555–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Radford AN (2005) Group-specific vocal signatures and neighbour–stranger discrimination in the cooperatively breeding green woodhoopoe. Animal Behav 70:1227–1234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Riebel K, Slater PJB (1998) Male chaffinches (Fringilla coelebs) can copy calls from a tape tutor. J Ornithol 139:353–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ríos-Chelén AA, Macias-García C (2004) Flight display song of the vermilion flycatcher. Wilson Bull 116:360–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ríos-Chelén AA, Macías Garcia C (2007) Responses of a sub-oscine bird during playback: effects of different song variants and breeding period. Behav Processes 74:319–325PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Ríos Chelén AA, Macías Garcia C, Riebel K (2005) Variation in the song of a sub-oscine, the vermilion flycatcher. Behaviour 142:1121–1138Google Scholar
  58. Ríos-Chelén AA, Jefferson AG, Torres R, Serrano-Pinto M, D’Alba L, Macías Garcia C (2008) Intra-specific brood parasitism revealed by DNA micro-satellite analyses in a sub-oscine bird, the vermilion flycatcher. Rev Chilena de Hist Nat 81:21–31Google Scholar
  59. Ríos-Chelén AA, McDonald AN, Berger A, Perry AC, Krakauer AH, Patricelli GL (2017) Do birds vocalize at higher pitch in noise, or is it a matter of measurement? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 71:29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Ríos-Chelén AA, Cuatianquiz-Lima C, Bautista A, Martínez-Gómez M (2018) No reliable evidence for immediate noise-induced song flexibility in a suboscine. Urban Ecosyst 21:15–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rivera-Cáceres K, Macías Garcia C, Quirós-Guerrero E, Ríos-Chelén AA (2011) An interactive playback experiment shows song bout size discrimination in the suboscine vermilion flycatcher (Pyrocephalus rubinus). Ethology 117:1120–1127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Robertson BC (1996) Vocal mate recognition in a monogamous, flock-forming bird, the silvereye, Zosterops lateralis. Animal Behav 51:303–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Robisson P, Aubin T, Bremond J-C (1993) Individuality in the voice of the emperor penguin Aptenodytes forsteri: adaptation to a noisy environment. Ethology 94:279–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Roper JJ (2005) Sexually distinct songs in the duet of the sexually monomorphic rufous hornero. J Field Ornithol 76:235–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Runciman D, Zann RA, Murray ND (2005) Geographic and temporal variation of the male zebra finch distance call. Ethology 111:367–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Seddon N, Tobias JA (2005) Duets defend mates in a suboscine passerine, the warbling antbird (Hypocnemis cantator). Behav Ecol 17:73–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Seddon N, Tobias JA (2010) Character displacement from the receiver’s perspective: species and mate recognition despite convergent signals in suboscine birds. Proc R Soc B 277:2475–2483PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Sieving KE, Hetrick SA, Avery ML (2010) The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication. Oikos 119:264–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Smith WJ (1967) Displays of the vermilion flycatcher (Pyrocephalus rubinus). Condor 69:601–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Smith WJ (1970) Courtship and territorial displaying in the vermilion flycatcher, Pyrocephalus rubinus. Condor 72:488–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Suzuki TN (2014) Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Animal Behav 87:59–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Taoka M, Sato T, Kamada T, Okumura H (1989) Sexual dimorphism of chatter-calls and vocal sex recognition in Leach’s storm-petrels (Oceanodroma leucorhoa). Auk 106:498–501Google Scholar
  73. Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  74. Tobias J, Brawn JD, Brumfield RT, Derryberry EP, Kirschel AN, Seddon N (2012) The importance of suboscine birds as study systems in ecology and evolution. Ornitol Neotrop 23:161–174Google Scholar
  75. Trainer JM, McDonald DB (1993) Vocal repertoire of the long-tailed manakin and its relation to male-male cooperation. Condor 95:769–781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Venuto V, Ferraiuolo V, Bottoni L, Massa R (2001) Distress call in six species of African Poicephalus parrots. Ethol Ecol Evol 13:49–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Vicario DS, Naqvi NH, Raksin JN (2001) Sex differences in discrimination of vocal communication signals in a songbird. Animal Behav 61:805–817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Vignal C, Mathevon N, Mottin S (2008) Mate recognition by female zebra finch: analysis of individuality in male call and first investigations on female decoding process. Behav Process 77:191–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wanker R, Apcin J, Jennerjahn B, Waibel B (1998) Discrimination of different social companions in spectacled parrotlets (Forpus conspicillatus): evidence for individual vocal recognition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 43:197–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wright TF (1996) Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proc R Soc Lond B 263:867–872CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Wright TF, Dorin M (2001) Pair duets in the yellow-naped amazon (Psittaciformes: Amazona auropalliata): responses to playbacks of different dialects. Ethology 107:111–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Wright TF, Wilkinson GS (2001) Population genetic structure and vocal dialects in an amazon parrot. Proc R Soc B 268:609–616PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. Yoneda T, Okanoya K (1991) Ontogeny of sexually dimorphic distance calls in Bengalese finches (Lonchura domestica). J Ethol 9:41–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Deutsche Ornithologen-Gesellschaft e.V. 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centro Tlaxcala de Biología de la ConductaUniversidad Autónoma de TlaxcalaTlaxcalaMexico
  2. 2.Escuela de BiologíaBenemérita Universidad Autónoma de PueblaPueblaMexico

Personalised recommendations