Journal of Ornithology

, Volume 153, Supplement 1, pp 97–104 | Cite as

Socialized sub-groups in a temporary stable Raven flock?

  • Anna Braun
  • Thomas Walsdorff
  • Orlaith N. Fraser
  • Thomas Bugnyar
Review

Abstract

A complex social life serves as one of the main driving forces behind the evolution of higher cognitive abilities in vertebrates. In birds, however, data are primarily derived from captive animals, which strongly contrast with free-flying birds in terms of the number of interaction partners as well as available space. In captivity, Common Raven Corvus corax, nonbreeder groups show strong social bonds and complex tactical manoeuvring, whereas wild non-breeders are thought to resemble anonymous aggregations. Over 2 years, we observed a free-flying population of Ravens that visits a game park in the northern Alps. We here focus on the daily fission–fusion dynamics, individual spacing, and the influence of spacing on the birds’ agonistic and affiliative behaviour. The composition of marked Ravens in the local population changed slowly but constantly, although often remaining stable for several weeks. Birds only flocked for feeding, mobbing and roosting, and spent the rest of the day in loose aggregations, characterised by temporary small subgroups of 2–5 individuals. Aggression was high during crowd foraging but low outside of a feeding context. Affiliative behaviours, such as sitting within reaching distance, allo-preening and social play, were observed particularly in the small subgroups. These findings suggest that Raven aggregations are not as unstructured as previously thought. Birds may spend time and/or interact affilliatively with multiple individuals during the day. This, along with temporary stability in group composition, provides the opportunity for social relationships to develop, and enables the existence of socialised subgroups within free-flying Raven aggregations.

Keywords

Common Raven Fission–fusion dynamics Group life Complex societies Corvids 

References

  1. Amici F, Aureli F, Call J (2008) Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioural flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Curr Biol 18:1415–1419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J, Chapman CA, Connor R, Di Fiore A, Dunbar RIM, Henzi SP, Holekamp K, Korstjens AH, Layton R, Lee P, Lehmann J, Manson JH, Ramos-Fernandez G, Strier KB, van Schaik CP (2008) Fission-fusion dynamics, new research frameworks. Curr Anthropol 49:627–654CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barrett L, Henzi P, Dunbar R (2003) Primate cognition: from ‘what now?’ to ‘what if?’ Trends Cogn Sci 7:494–497Google Scholar
  4. Beauchamp G (1999) The evolution of communal roosting in birds: origin and secondary losses. Behav Ecol 10:675–687CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beauchamp G (2010) Relaxed predation risk reduces but does not eliminate sociality in birds. Biol Lett 6:472–474PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boarman WI (2003) Managing a subsidized predator population: reducing common raven predation on desert tortoises. Environ Manag 32:205–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP (2003) Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Anim Behav 65:479–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP (2007) Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). J Comp Psychol 121:372–379PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bugnyar T (2011) Knower-guesser differentiation in ravens: others’ viewpoints matter. Proc R Soc Lond B 278:634–640CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bugnyar T, Heinrich B (2005) Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:1641–1646CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bugnyar T, Heinrich B (2006) Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors. Anim Cogn 9:369–376PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2002) Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it ‘tactical’ deception? Anim Behav 64:185–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Byrne RW, Bates LA (2007a) Brain evolution: when is a group not a group? Curr Biol 17:R884Google Scholar
  14. Byrne RW, Bates LA (2007b) Sociality, evolution and cognition. Curr Biol 17:R714–R723PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Byrne RW, Whiten A (1988) Machiavellian intelligence. Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  16. Caffrey C (2000) Marking crows. N Am Bird Bander 26:146–148Google Scholar
  17. Canestrari D, Marcos JM, Baglione V (2005) Effect of parentage and relatedness on the individual contribution to cooperative chick care in carrion crows Corvus corone corone. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:422–428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Carlisle TR, Zahavi A (1986) Helping at the nest, allofeeding and social status in immature arabian babblers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:339–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Connor RC, Heithaus MR, Barre LM (1999) Superalliance of bottlenose dolphins. Nature 397:571–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dall SRX, Wright J (2009) Rich pickings near large communal roosts favor ‘gang’ foraging by juvenile common ravens, Corvus corax. PLoS ONE 4:e4530PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dally JM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2006) The behaviour and evolution of cache protection and pilferage. Anim Behav 72:13–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. de Waal FBM, Tyack PL (2003) Preface. In: de Waal FBM, Tyack PL (eds) Animal social complexity: intelligence, culture, and individualized societies. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp ix–xivGoogle Scholar
  23. Diamond J, Bond AB (2003) A comparative analysis of social play in birds. Behaviour 140:1091–1115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Drack G, Kotrschal K (1995) Aktivitätsmuster und Spiel von freilebenden Kolkraben Corvus corax im inneren Almtal/Oberösterreich. Monticula 7:159–174Google Scholar
  25. Dunbar RIM (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6:178–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eklow A (1988) The behaviour of ravens Corvus corax, in fresh snow. Var Fagelvarld 47:89–90Google Scholar
  27. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2001) Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:443–446PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Emery NJ, Dally JM, Clayton NS (2004) Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) use cognitive strategies to protect their caches from thieving conspecifics. Anim Cogn 7:37–43PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Emery NJ, Seed AM, von Bayern AM, Clayton NS (2007) Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 362:489–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T (2010a) The quality of social relationships in ravens. Anim Behav 79:927–933CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T (2010b) Do ravens show consolation? Responses to distressed others. PLoS ONE 5:e10605PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T (2011) Ravens reconcile after aggressive conflicts with valuabe partners. PLoS ONE 6:e18118PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fraser ON, Bugnyar T (2012) Reciprocity of agonistic support in ravens. Anim Behav 83:171–177PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gwinner E (1964) Untersuchungen über das Ausdrucks- und Sozialverhalten des Kolkraben (Corvus corax corax L.). Z Tierpsychol 21:657–748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gwinner E (1966) Über einige Bewegungsspiele des Kolkraben (Corvus corax L.). Z Tierpsychol 23:28–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Haffer J (1993) Corvidae—Rabenvögel. In: Glutz von Blotzheim UN (ed) Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas. Aula, Wiesbaden, pp 1947–2022Google Scholar
  37. Heinrich B (1988) Winter foraging at carcasses by three sympatric corvids, with emphasis on recruitment by the raven, Corvus corax. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:141–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Heinrich B (1989) Ravens in winter. Simon & Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  39. Heinrich B (1994a) Dominance and weight changes in the common raven, Corvus corax. Anim Behav 48:1463–1465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Heinrich B (1994b) Age and mouth color in common ravens. Condor 94:549–550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Heinrich B (2011) Conflict, cooperation and cognition in the common raven. Adv Stud behav 43:189–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Heinrich B, Smolker R (1998) Play in common ravens (Corvus corax). In: Bekoff M, Byers JA (eds) Animal play: evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 27–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Heinrich B, Kaye D, Knight T, Schaumburg K (1994) Dispersal and association among common ravens. Condor 96:545–551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Hinde RA (1976) Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11:11–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Holekamp KE, Cooper SM, Katona CI, Berry NA, Frank LG, Smale L (1997) Patterns of association among female spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). J Mammal 78:55–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Huber B (1991) Bildung, Alterszusammensetzung und Sozialstruktur von Gruppen nichtbrütender Kolkraben (Corvus corax L.). Metelen Schr Natursch 2:45–59Google Scholar
  47. Marzluff JM, Heinrich B, Marzluff CS (1996) Raven roosts are mobile information centres. Anim Behav 51:89–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McComb K, Moss C, Durant SM, Baker L, Sayialel S (2001) Matriarchs as repositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science 292:491–494PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Parker PG, Waite TA, Heinrich B, Marzluff JM (1994) Do common ravens share ephemeral food resources with kin? DNA fingerprinting evidence. Anim Behav 48:1085–1093CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Paz-y-Mino CG, Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP (2004) Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature 430:778–781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rösner S, Selva N (2005) Use of the bait-marking method to estimate the territory size of scavenging birds: a case study on ravens Corvus corax. Wildl Biol 11:183–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rösner S, Selva N, Müller T, Pugacewicz E, Laudet F (2005) Raven Corvus corax ecology in a primeval temperate forest. In: Jerzak L, Kavanagh BP, Tryjanowski P (eds) Corvids of Poland. Bogucki Wyd, Nuak, Poznan, pp 385–405Google Scholar
  53. Seed AM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2007) Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Curr Biol 17:152–158PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Shoemaker CM, Phillips RS (2011) Observation of ground roosting by American crows. Wilson J Ornithol 123:185–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (1987) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  56. Stahler D, Heinrich B, Douglas S (2002) Common ravens, Corvus corax, preferentially associate with grey wolves, Canis lupus, as a foraging strategy in winter. Anim Behav 64:283–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Stiehl RB (1978) Aspects of the ecology of the common raven in Harney Basin, Oregon. PhD thesis, Portland State UniversityGoogle Scholar
  58. Symington MM (1990) Fission-fusion social organization in Ateles and Pan. Int J Primatol 11:47–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Vander Wall SB, Balda RP (1977) Coadaptations of the clark’s nutcracker and the pinon pine for efficient seed harvest and dispersal. Ecol Monogr 47:89–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Webb WC, Boarman WI, Rotenberry JT (2009) Movements of juvenile Common ravens in an arid landscape. J Wildlife Manag 73:72–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wright J, Stone RE, Brown N (2003) Communal roosts as structured information centres in the raven, Corvus corax. J Anim Ecol 72:1003–1014CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Dt. Ornithologen-Gesellschaft e.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anna Braun
    • 1
    • 2
  • Thomas Walsdorff
    • 1
    • 3
  • Orlaith N. Fraser
    • 1
  • Thomas Bugnyar
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.University of ViennaViennaAustria
  2. 2.Konrad Lorenz Research StationGruenauAustria
  3. 3.Université catholique de LouvainLouvain-la-NeuveBelgium

Personalised recommendations