Advertisement

Primates

, Volume 59, Issue 4, pp 377–384 | Cite as

The influence of kinship and dominance hierarchy on grooming partner choice in free-ranging Macaca mulatta brevicaudus

  • Cheng-Feng Wu
  • Zhi-Jie Liao
  • Cedric Sueur
  • John Chih Mun Sha
  • Jie Zhang
  • Peng Zhang
Original Article

Abstract

In group-living animals, individuals do not interact uniformly with their conspecifics. Among primates, such heterogeneity in partner choice can be discerned from affiliative grooming patterns. While the preference for selecting close kin as grooming partners is ubiquitous across the primate order, the selection of higher-ranking non-kin individuals as grooming partners is less common. We studied a group of provisioned rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta brevicaudus) on Hainan Island, China, to examine rank-related benefits of grooming exchanges and the influence of kin relationships. We tested four hypotheses based on Seyfarth’s model: (1) there will be kin preference in grooming relationships; (2) grooming between non-kin individuals will be directed up the dominance rank; (3) grooming between non-kin individuals will reduce aggression from higher-ranking ones; and (4) non-kin individuals will spend more time grooming with adjacent ranked ones. We found that grooming relationships between kin individuals were stronger than those between non-kin individuals. For non-kin relationships, lower-ranking individuals received less aggression from higher-ranking ones through grooming; a benefit they could not derive through grooming exchanges with individuals related by kinship. Individuals spent more time grooming adjacent higher-ranking non-kin individuals and higher-ranking individuals also received more grooming from non-kin individuals. Our results supported Seyfarth’s model for predicting partner choice between non-kin individuals. For relationships between kin individuals, we found results that were not consistent with prediction for the exchanges of aggression and grooming, indicating the importance to control for the influence of kinship in future studies.

Keywords

Aggression Dominance rank Kin preference Rhesus macaque Seyfarth’s model 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31470456, 31772472, 31711540021), Research Project of Guangzhou Zoo (2018-01). We are grateful to the Nanwan Monkey Park and Nanwan Nature Reserve for Rhesus Macaque for granting permission and support to carry out this research. We thank Qi-xuan Hong, Xiao-chan Yan, and Bo-jun Liu for their assistance in the field. We thank Hang Ning and Christy Lee Ward for their help in English proofreading. We thank all members of the Anthropology Department and School of Life Sciences in Sun Yat-sen University, China for their support.

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Balasubramaniam KN, Dittmar K, Berman CM, Butovskaya M, Cooper MA, Majolo B, Ogawa H, Schino G, Thierry B, de Waal FBM (2012) Hierarchical steepness and phylogenetic models: phylogenetic signals in Macaca. Anim Behav 83:1207–1218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berman CM, Thierry B (2010) Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaques. Behaviour 147:1863–1887CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berman CM, Ogawa H, Ionica C, Yin H, Li J (2008) Variation in kin bias over time in a group of Tibetan macaques at Huangshan, China: contest competition, time constraints or risk response? Behaviour 145:863–896CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Freeman LC (2002) UCINET for windows: software for social network analysis. Analytic Technologies, HarvardGoogle Scholar
  6. Broom M, Cannings C (2013) A dynamic network population model with strategic link formation governed by individual preferences. J Theor Biol 335:160–168CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Call J, Judge PG, de Waal FBM (1996) Influence of kinship and spatial density on reconciliation and grooming in rhesus monkeys. Am J Primatol 39:35–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cheney DL, Silk JB, Seyfarth RM (2016) Network connections, dyadic bonds and fitness in wild female baboons. R Soc Open Sci 3:160255CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Cooper MA, Bernstein IS (2008) Evaluating dominance styles in Assamese and rhesus macaques. Int J Primatol 29:225–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. David D, David K, Snijders TAB (2007) Sensitivity of MRQAP tests to collinearity and autocorrelation conditions. Psychometrika 72:563–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. de Waal FB (1977) The organization of agonistic relations within two captive groups of Java-monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 44:225–282CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. de Waal FB, Luttrell LM (1986) The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatol 46:215–234CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Demaria C, Thierry B (2001) A comparative study of reconciliation in rhesus and Tonkean macaques. Behaviour 138:397–410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dunbar RIM (1992a) Time: a hidden constraint on the behavioural ecology of baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 31:35–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dunbar RIM (1992b) Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J Hum Evol 22:469–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fraser ON, Schino G, Aureli F (2008) Components of relationship quality in chimpanzees. Ethology 114:834–843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ginsberg JR, Young TP (1992) Measuring association between individuals or groups in behavioural studies. Anim Behav 44, Part 2 2:377–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hamilton WD (1964a) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. J Theor Biol 7:17–52CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Hamilton WD (1964b) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. J Theor Biol 7:1–16CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. He Z, Li G, Li G, Chen Z, Wang L (2011) Laboratory animal welfare and animal experimental science. Science Press, BeijingGoogle Scholar
  21. Jiang H, Liu Z, Yuan X, Wang H (1988) Activity habits of rhesus monkeys at Nanwan Peninsula of Hainan Island. Acta Theriol Sin 8:294–298Google Scholar
  22. Jiang XL, Wang YX, Ma SL (1991) Taxonomic revision and distribution of subspecies of rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) in China. Zool Res 12:241–247Google Scholar
  23. Jiang H, Lian J, Feng M, Wang J, Li Y (1998) Studies on population growth of Macaca mulatta at Nanwan, Hainan. Acta Theriol Sin 18:100–106Google Scholar
  24. Kapsalis E, Berman CM (1996) Models of affiliative relationships among free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): II. Testing predictions for three hypothesized organizing principles. Behaviour 133:1235–1263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Krackhardt D (1988) Predicting with networks: nonparametric multiple regression analysis of dyadic data. Soc Netw 10:359–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Maestripieri D (1999) Primate social organization, gestural repertoire size, and communication dynamics: a comparative study of macaques. In: King BJ (ed) The evolution of language: assessing the evidence from nonhuman primates. School American Research Pr, Santa Fe, pp 55–77Google Scholar
  27. Matheson MD, Bernstein IS (2000) Grooming, social bonding, and agonistic aiding in rhesus monkeys. Am J Primatol 51:177–186CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Nowak MA (2006) Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science 314:1560–1563CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Ohtsuki H, Hauert C, Lieberman E, Nowak MA (2006) A simple rule for the evolution of cooperation on graphs and social networks. Nature 441:502–505CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Perry S (1996) Female–female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Am J Primatol 40:167–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rowe N (1996) The pictorial guide to the living primates. Pogonias Press, East HamptonGoogle Scholar
  32. Schino G (2001) Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 62:265–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schino G, Ventura R, Troisi A (2003) Grooming among female Japanese macaques: distinguishing between reciprocation and interchange. Behav Ecol 14:887–891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schino G, Ventura R, Troisi A (2005) Grooming and aggression in captive Japanese macaques. Primates 46:207–209CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Schino G, Di Giuseppe F, Visalberghi E (2009) Grooming, rank, and agonistic support in tufted capuchin monkeys. Am J Primatol 71:101–105CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Seyfarth RM (1977) A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol 65:671–698CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Silk JB (1982) Altruism among female Macaca radiata: explanations and analysis of patterns of grooming and coalition formation. Behaviour 79:162–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2006) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) II. Variation in the quality and stability of social bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:197–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Smith D, McDonough J (2005) Mitochondrial DNA variation in Chinese and Indian rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Am J Primatol 65:1–25CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Snyder-Mackler N, Kohn JN, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP, Wilson ME, Tung J (2016) Social status drives social relationships in groups of unrelated female rhesus macaques. Anim Behav 111:307–317CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Sueur C, Petit O, De Marco A, Jacobs AT, Watanabe K, Thierry B (2011) A comparative network analysis of social style in macaques. Anim Behav 82:845–852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Thierry B (2007) Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. Evol Anthropol 16:224–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ventura R, Majolo B, Koyama NF, Hardie S, Schino G (2006) Reciprocation and interchange in wild Japanese macaques: grooming, cofeeding, and agonistic support. Am J Primatol 68:1138–1149CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Voelkl B, Kasper C (2009) Social structure of primate interaction networks facilitates the emergence of cooperation. Biol Lett 5:462–464CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Wang BS, Wang ZL, Tian JD, Cui ZW, Lu JQ (2015) Establishment of a microsatellite set for noninvasive paternity testing in free-ranging Macaca mulatta tcheliensis in Mount Taihangshan area, Jiyuan, China. Zool Stud 54:81–90Google Scholar
  46. Wu J (2014) minque: an R package for linear mixed model analyses. R package version 1.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=minque
  47. Xia D, Li J, Garber PA, Matheson MD, Sun B, Zhu Y (2013) Grooming reciprocity in male Tibetan macaques. Am J Primatol 75:1009–1020CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Zhang P, Lyu M, Wu C, Chu Y, Han N, Yang D, Hu K (2016) Variation in body mass and morphological characters in Macaca mulatta brevicaudus from Hainan, China. Am J Primatol 78:679–698CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer Japan KK, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Martin Hall, Anthropology Department, School of Sociology and AnthropologySun Yat-sen UniversityGuangzhouChina
  2. 2.Provincial Key Laboratory of Human Evolution and ArchaeometrySun Yat-sen UniversityGuangzhouChina
  3. 3.College of Life SciencesSun Yat-sen UniversityGuangzhouChina
  4. 4.Université de Strasbourg, CNRS, IPHC, UMR 7178StrasbourgFrance
  5. 5.Nanwan Monkey IsletLingshui TownChina

Personalised recommendations