Primates

pp 1–11 | Cite as

Extractive foraging and tool-aided behaviors in the wild Nicobar long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis umbrosus)

  • Arijit Pal
  • Honnavalli N. Kumara
  • Partha Sarathi Mishra
  • Avadhoot D. Velankar
  • Mewa Singh
Original Article

Abstract

Macaques possess a repertoire of extractive foraging techniques that range from complex manipulation to tool-aided behaviors, to access food items that increase their foraging efficiency substantially. However, the complexity and composition of such techniques vary considerably between species and even between populations. In the present study, we report seven such complex manipulative behaviors that include six extractive foraging behaviors, and teeth flossing, in a population of Nicobar long-tailed macaques. The apparent purpose of these behaviors was an extraction of encased food, processing food, foraging hidden invertebrates, and dental flossing. Among these behaviors, three behaviors viz. wrapping, wiping, and teeth-flossing were tool-aided behaviors, where macaques used both natural and synthetic materials as tools. Occasionally macaques also modified those tools prior to their use. The substrate use patterns of leaf rubbing and teeth flossing were similar to that observed in other macaques. The spontaneous tool modification to perform wrapping was a first time observation. These observations suggest that Nicobar long-tailed macaques have a high level of sensorimotor intelligence which helps to evolve such innovative foraging solutions.

Keywords

Bush beating Flossing Husking Substrate use Wiping Wrapping 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The present research was supported by the Department of Science and Technology, Government of India (Grant no. SR/SO/AS-49/2011) to HNK and INSPIRE fellowship, Department of Science and Technology, Government of India (Grant no. A.20020/11/97-IFD.DT.31.03.2010) to A. Pal. We thank Principal Chief Conservator of Forests, Port Blair, Andaman and Nicobar islands for permitting us to carry out the field work (Permit no. CWLW/WL/134/566). We thank the local people of Campbell Bay for being helpful throughout the study. MS and PSM thank SERB for the award of a J.C. Bose Fellowship during which this article was prepared. We also acknowledge the anonymous reviewers and the handling editor whose comments helped enhance the quality of this article.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

This study was approved by the Ethical Committee for Animal Research of Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural History, Coimbatore. It also adhered to the guidelines and principles of the Ministry of Environment, Forest and Climate Change, Government of India. Further, permission to carry out this study was granted by the Andaman and Nicobar Forest Department (Permit no. CWLW/WL/134/566).

Supplementary material

Video 1 An adult male performing coconut pounding to break outer shell of a dehusked coconut (MP4 15882 kb)

Video 2 A subadult male employing tool-aided food rubbing by rubbing dry leaves over a cashew nut to remove latex (MP4 47576 kb)

Video 3 A left-hand amputee adult male modifying a tool by detaching it from tree twigs and wrapping it over dehusked coconut shell (MP4 455270 kb)

Video 4 An adult female cleaning sand from food items by employing food rubbing by hand technique (MP4 27847 kb)

Video 5 A subadult male flossing teeth with a feather (MP4 16030 kb)

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–265CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bertrand M (1966) Training without reward: traditional training of pig-tailed macaques as coconut harvesters. Science 155:484–486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Carpenter A (1887) Monkeys opening oysters. Nature 36:53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chiang M (1967) Use of tools by wild macaque monkeys in Singapore. Nature 214:1258–1259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Comins JA, Russ BE, Humbert KA, Hauser MD (2011) Innovative coconut-opening in a semi free-ranging rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta): a case report on behavioral propensities. J Ethol 29:187–189CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Dunba RIM (1977) Feeding ecology of gelada baboons: a preliminary report. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Primate ecology. Academic, London, pp 251–273Google Scholar
  7. Erwin JM, Southwik CH (1992) Population, habitat and conservation status of Macaca mourus, Macaca tonkeana and their putative hybrids. Trop Biodivers 1:31–48Google Scholar
  8. Ewer RF (1968) Ethology of mammals. Plenum, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Goodall JVL, Goodall HVL (1966) Use of tools by the Egyptian vulture, Neophron percnopterus. Nature 212:1468–1469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gumert M, Kluck M, Malaivijitnond S (2009) The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand. Am J Primatol 71:594–608CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gumert M, Hoong LK, Malaivijitnond S (2011) Sex differences in the stone tool-use behavior of a wild population of Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea). Am J Primatol 73:1239–1249CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hall KRL, Schaller GB (1964) Tool-using behavior of the California sea otter. J Mammal 45:287–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hauser MD, Marier P (1993) Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors. Behav Ecol 4:194–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hohmann G (1988) A case of simple tool use in wild lion tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). Primates 29:565–567CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Izawa K (1982) Nihonzaru no Seitai: Gousetsu no Hakusanni Yaseiwo Tou. [Ecology of Japanese Macaques: Considering the Wilderness in the Heavy-Snowing Hakusan Mountains], (In Japanese). Tokyo, Doubutsu-ShaGoogle Scholar
  16. Jaman MF, Huffman MA (2013) The effect of urban and rural habitats and resource type an activity budgets of commensal rhesus macaques (Macaca radiata) in Bangladesh. Primates 54:49–59CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Kawai M (1965) Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6:1–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kawamura S (1959) The process of sub-culture propagation among Japanese macaques. Primates 2:43–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. King BJ (1986) Extractive foraging and the evolution of primate intelligence. Hum Evol 1:361–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kuruvilla GP (1980) Ecology of the bonnet macaque (Macaca radiata) with special reference to feeding habits. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc 75:976–988Google Scholar
  21. Leca JB, Gunst N, Huffman MA (2010) Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Anim Behav 79:117–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Malaivijitnond S, Lekprayoon C, Tandavanitj N, Panha S, Cheewatham C, Hamada Y (2007) Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Am J Primatol 69:227–233CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Melin AD, Young HC, Mosdossy KW, Fedigan LM (2014) Seasonality, extractive foraging and the evolution of primate sensorimotor intelligence. J Hum Evol 71:77–86CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Menard N (2004) Do ecological factors explain variation in social organization. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 237–266Google Scholar
  25. Menzel EW, Wyers EJ (1981) Cognitive aspects of foraging behaviour. In: Kamil AC, Sargent TD (eds) Foraging behavior: ecological, ethological and psychological approaches. Garland STPM, New York, pp 355–377Google Scholar
  26. Moura ADA, Lee PC (2004) Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest. Science 306:1909CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. O’brien TG, Kinnaird MF (1997) Behavior, diet and movents of Sulawesi crested black macaques (Macaca nigra). Int J Primatol 18:321–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Parker ST, Gibson KR (1977) Object manipulation, tool use, and sensorimotor intelligence as adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. J Hum Evol 6:623–641CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sinha A (1997) Complex tool manufacture by a wild bonnet macaque. Folia Primatol 68:23–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sugiyama Y, Koman J (1979) Tool-using and -making behavior of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20:513–524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Supriatna J, Froehlich JW, Erwin JM, Southwick CH (1992) Population, habitat and conservation status of Macaca maurus, Macaca tonkena and their putative hybrids. Trop Biodivers 1:31–48Google Scholar
  32. Suzuki A (1965) An ecological study of wild Japanese monkeys in snowy areas: focused on their food habits. Primates 6:31–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Tan AWY (2017) From play to proficiency: the ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective. J Comp Psychol 131:89–114CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Tan A, Tan SH, Vyas D, Malaivijitnond S, Gumert MD (2015) There is more than one way to crack an oyster: identifying variation in Burmese long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis aurea) stone-tool use. PLoS One 10(2):e0124733. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124733 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Tan AWY, Luncz L, Haslman M, Malaivijitnond S, Gumert M (2016) Complex processing of prickly pear cactus (Opuntia sp.) by free ranging long-tailed macaques: preliminary analysis for hierarchical organisation. Primates 57:141–147CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Troscianko J, Bluff LA, Rutz C (2008) Grass-stem tool use in new Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Ardea 96:283–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Umapathy G, Singh M, Mohnot SM (2003) Status and distribution of Macaca fascicularis umbrosa in the Nicobar Islands, India. Int J Primatol 24:281–293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. van Schaik CP, Deaner RO, Merrill MY (1999) The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J Hum Evol 36:719–741CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Velankar AD, Kumara HN, Pal A, Mishra PS, Singh M (2016) Population recovery of Nicobar long-tailed macaque Macaca fascicularis umbrosus following a tsunami in the Nicobar Islands, India. PLoS One 11(2):e0148205. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0148205 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Visalberghi E, Fragaszy DM (1990) Do monkey ape? In: Parker ST, Gibson KR (eds) “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 247–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Watanabe K, Urasopon N, Malaivijitnond S (2007) Long-tailed macaques use human hair as dental floss. Am J Primatol 69:940–944CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Watts DP (2008) Tool use by chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale national park, Uganda. Int J Primatol 29:83–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wheatley BP (1988) Cultural behavior and extractive foraging in Macaca fascicularis. Curr Anthropol 29:516–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Yamakoshi G (2004) Evolution of complex feeding techniques in primates: is this the origin of great ape intelligence? In: Russon AE, Begun DR (eds) The evolution of thought: evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 140–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer Japan KK 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Arijit Pal
    • 1
    • 2
  • Honnavalli N. Kumara
    • 1
  • Partha Sarathi Mishra
    • 1
    • 3
  • Avadhoot D. Velankar
    • 1
    • 2
  • Mewa Singh
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural HistoryCoimbatoreIndia
  2. 2.Manipal UniversityManipalIndia
  3. 3.Bharathiar UniversityCoimbatoreIndia
  4. 4.Biopsychology Laboratory and Institution of ExcellenceUniversity of MysoreMysoreIndia
  5. 5.Organismal Biology UnitJawaharlal Nehru Centre for Advance Scientific ResearchBangaloreIndia

Personalised recommendations