Advertisement

Primates

, Volume 53, Issue 3, pp 215–219 | Cite as

Bonobos apparently search for a lost member injured by a snare

  • Nahoko Tokuyama
  • Besao Emikey
  • Batuafe Bafike
  • Batuafe Isolumbo
  • Bahanande Iyokango
  • Mbangi N. Mulavwa
  • Takeshi FuruichiEmail author
News and Perspectives

Abstract

This is the first report to demonstrate that a large mixed-sex party of bonobos travelled a long distance to return to the location of a snare apparently to search for a member that had been caught in it. An adult male was caught in a metallic snare in a swamp forest at Wamba, Luo Scientific Reserve, Democratic Republic of the Congo. After he escaped from the snare by breaking a sapling to which the snare was attached, other members of his party assisted him by unfastening the snare from lianas in which it was caught and licked his wound and tried to remove the snare from his fingers. In the late afternoon, they left him in the place where he was stuck in the liana and travelled to the dry forest where they usually spend the night. The next morning, they travelled back 1.8 km to revisit the location of the injured male. When they confirmed that he was no longer there, they returned to the dry forest to forage. This was unlike the usual ranging patterns of the party, suggesting that the bonobos travelled with the specific intention of searching for this injured individual who had been left behind. The incident described in this report likely occurred because bonobos usually range in a large mixed-sex party and try to maintain group cohesion as much as possible.

Keywords

Pan paniscus Wamba Snare Group cohesion Search lost member 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the Research Center for Ecology and Forestry, Ministry of Scientific Research, DRC and African Wildlife Foundation for helping our field research. We thank Dr. Chie Hashimoto, Mr. Gaku Ohashi, Dr. Koichiro Zamma, and Dr. Kazuhiko Hosaka for giving us valuable information. We thank Dr. Janet Nackoney and Dr. Andrew MacIntosh for their English editing and helpful comments. We give special thanks to Dr. Tetsuya Sakamaki for his great contribution to continuous observation of study groups and camp management at Wamba. We were supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Grants-in-aid for Scientific Research (22255007 to Furuichi), JSPS Asia-Africa Science Platform Program (2009-8 to Furuichi), JSPS Institutional Program for Young Researcher Overseas Visits (#37 to Primate Research Institute Kyoto University), Japan Ministry of the Environment Research and Technology Development Fund (D-1007 to Furuichi), US Fish and Wildlife Service Assistance Award (96200-0-G017 to African Wildlife Foundation), and AS-HOPE project (AS-23-004 to Tokuyama).

References

  1. Amati S, Babweteera F, Witting RM (2008) Snare removal by a chimpanzee of the Sonso community, Budongo Forest (Uganda). Pan Afr News 15:6–8Google Scholar
  2. Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: behavioral ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Furuichi T (1988) Biliano sumu moride: Africa, hito, pygmychimpanzee (in the forest of biliya: Africa, people, pygmy chimpanzees). Tokyo Kagaku Dojin, Tokyo (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  4. Furuichi T (1989) Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. Int J Primatol 10:173–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Furuichi T (1999) Seino shinka, hitono shinka (evolution of sex, evolution of man). Asahi News Paper, Tokyo (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  6. Furuichi T (2009) Factors underlying party size differences between chimpanzees and bonobos: a review and hypotheses for future study. Primates 50:197–209PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Furuichi T (2011) Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evol Anthropol 20:131–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Furuichi T, Idani G, Ihobe H, Hashimoto C, Tashiro Y, Sakamaki T, Mulavwa MN, Yangozene K, Kuroda S (2012) Long-term studies on wild bonobos at Wamba, Luo Scientific Reserve, D. R. Congo: towards the understanding of female life history in a male-philopatric species. In: Kappeler PM, Watts DP (eds) Long-term field studies of primates. Springer, Berlin, pp 413–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  10. Hashimoto C (1999) Snare injuries of chimpanzees in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda. Pan Afr News 6:20–22Google Scholar
  11. Hashimoto C, Furuichi T, Tashiro Y (2001) What factors affect the size of chimpanzee parties in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda? Examination of fruit abundance and number of estrous females. Int J Primatol 22:947–959CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Huffman MA (1989) Observations on the illness and consumption of a possibly medical plant Vermonia amygdalina by a wild chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 30:51–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Idani G (1990) Relations between unit-groups of bonobos at Wamba, Zaire: encounters and temporary fusions. Afr Study Monogr 11:153–186Google Scholar
  14. Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba. Primates 23:171–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kano T (1992) The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford University Press, StanfordGoogle Scholar
  16. Kano T, Bongoli L, Idani G, Hashimoto C (1996) The challenge of Wamba. Etica and Animali 8:68–74Google Scholar
  17. Kuroda S (1979) Grouping of the pygmy chimpanzees. Primates 20:161–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mulavwa MN, Yangozene K, Yamba-Yamba M, Motema-Salo B, Mwanza NN, Furuichi T (2010) Nest groups of wild bonobos at Wamba: selection of vegetation and tree species and relationships between nest group size and party size. Am J Primatol 72:575–586PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Nishida T (1968) The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates 9:167–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Nishida T, Kano T, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nakamura M (1999) Ethogram and ethnography of mahale chimpanzees. Anthropol Sci 107:141–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ohashi G, Matsuzawa T (2011) Deactivation of snares by wild chimpanzees. Primates 52:1–5PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Quiatt D, Reynolds V, Stokes EJ (2002) Snare injuries to chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at 10 study sites in east and west Africa. Afr J Ecol 40:303–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Reynolds V (2005) The chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: ecology, behaviour, and conservation. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Surbeck M, Mundry R, Hohmann G (2011) Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos (Pan paniscus). Proc R Soc B 278:590–598PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Waller JC, Reynolds V (2001) Limb injuries resulting from snares and traps in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Primates 42:135–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. White FJ (1988) Party composition and dynamics in Pan paniscus. Int J Primatol 9:179–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wrangham RW (1979) Sex differences in chimpanzee dispersion. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER (eds) The great apes. Benjamin/Cummings Publishing, Menlo Park, pp 481–489Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nahoko Tokuyama
    • 1
  • Besao Emikey
    • 2
  • Batuafe Bafike
    • 2
  • Batuafe Isolumbo
    • 2
  • Bahanande Iyokango
    • 2
  • Mbangi N. Mulavwa
    • 2
  • Takeshi Furuichi
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Primate Research Institute, Kyoto UniversityInuyamaJapan
  2. 2.Research Center for Ecology and Forestry, Ministry of Scientific ResearchMabaliDemocratic Republic of the Congo

Personalised recommendations