Advertisement

Primates

, Volume 47, Issue 4, pp 393–396 | Cite as

Diurnal distribution of loud calls in sympatric wild indris (Indri indri) and ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata): implications for call functions

  • Thomas Geissmann
  • Thomas Mutschler
Short Communication

Abstract

We carried out a short study on the diurnal call distribution of two sympatric lemurs in the Réserve Naturelle Intégrale Zahamena (eastern Madagascar). Whereas indris (Indri) song bouts were clearly concentrated in the early morning hours, the roar/shriek choruses of ruffed lemurs (Varecia) exhibited a much more even distribution throughout the day. These differences in distribution pattern support earlier claims that indri song bouts are more likely to serve territorial functions, whereas ruffed lemur loud calls may serve both spacing and/or alarm call functions.

Keywords

Calls Diurnal distribution Function of calls Song Madagascar 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank the Direction des Eaux et Forêts for permission to conduct research in the Réserve Naturelle Intégrale Zahamena and Mamirina Rakotorahalany Josoa, Rajaonarivo Aimé Bernard and M. Ratodymanana for assistance in the field. Two anonymous reviewers kindly read and commented on our manuscript. T.G. was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.

References

  1. Brown TJ, Handford P (2003) Why birds sing at dawn: the role of consistent song transmission. Ibis 145:120–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Byg A, Balslev H (2001) Diversity and use of palms in Zahamena, eastern Madagascar. Biodiv Conserv 10:951–970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dabelsteen T, Mathevon N (2002) Why do songbirds sing intensively at dawn? A test of the acoustic transmission hypothesis. Acta Ethol 4:65-72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ganzhorn JU (1998) Nested patterns of species composition and their implications for lemur biogeography in Madagascar. Folia Primatol 69(Suppl 1):332–341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Geissmann T (2000) Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 103–123Google Scholar
  6. Geissmann T (2002) Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biol Rev 77:57–76CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Green S, Marler P (1979) The analysis of animal communication. In: Marler P, Vandenberg J (eds) Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, vol 3. Plenum Press, New York, pp 73–158Google Scholar
  8. Haimoff EH (1986) Convergence in the duetting of monogamous old World monkeys. J Hum Evol 15:51–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kress JH, Conley JM, Eaglen RH, Ibanez AE (1978) The behaviour of Lemur variegatus Kerr 1792. Z Tierpsychol 48:87–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Macedonia JM, Taylor LL (1985) Subspecific divergence in a loud call of the ruffed lemur (Varecia variegata). Am J Primatol 9:295–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mittermeier RA, Tattersall I, Konstant WR, Meyers DM, Mast RB (1994) Lemurs of Madagascar. Conservation International, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  12. Oliver WLR, O’Connor SM (1980) Circadian distribution of Indri indri group vocalisations: a short period sampling at two study sites near Perinet, eastern Madagascar. Dodo J Jersey Wildl Preserv Trust 17:19–27Google Scholar
  13. Pereira ME, Seeligson ML, Macedonia JM (1988) The behavioral repertoire of the black-and-white ruffed lemur, Varecia variegata variegata (Primates: Lemuridae). Folia Primatol 51:1–32CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Petter J-J (1962) Recherches sur l’écologie et l’éthologie des Lémuriens Malgaches. Mém Mus Natl Hist Nat, Sér A, Zool 27 (1):1–146, + 26 platesGoogle Scholar
  15. Petter J-J, Albignac R, Rumpler Y (1977) Faune de Madagascar vol 44: Mammifères Lémuriens (Primates Prosimiens). ORSTOM/CNRS, ParisGoogle Scholar
  16. Petter J-J, Charles-Dominique P (1979) Vocal comunication in prosimians. In: Doyle GA, Martin RD (eds) The study of prosimian behavior. Academic, New York, pp 247–305Google Scholar
  17. Pollock JI (1975) Field observations on Indri indri: a preliminary report. In: Tattersall I, Sussman RW (eds) Lemur biology. Plenum Press, New York, pp 287–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Pollock JI (1986) The song of the indris (Indri indri; Primates: Lemuroidea): natural history, form, and function. Int J Primatol 7:225–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Rigamonti MM (1993) Home range and diet in red ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata rubra) on the Masoala peninsula, Madagascar. In: Kappeler PM, Ganzhorn JU (eds) Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Plenum Press, New York, London, pp 25–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Thalmann U, Geissmann T, Simona A, Mutschler T (1993) The indris of Anjanaharibe-Sud, northeastern Madagascar. Int J Primatol 14:357–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Traber SY (2004) Raumnutzung und Aktivität bei Roten Varis (Varecia variegata rubra) und Weisskopfmakis (Eulemur fulvus albifrons) im Kleinaffenhaus und der Masoala-Halle des Zoo Zürich. Diploma Thesis, Anthropological Institute, Zürich University, pp 49Google Scholar
  22. Waser PM, Brown CH (1984) Is there a “sound window” for primate communication? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 15:73–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Waser PM, Brown CH (1986) Habitat acoustics and primate communication. Am J Primatol 10:135–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. White FJ (1991) Social organization, feeding ecology, and reproductive strategy of ruffed lemurs, Varecia variegata. In: Ehara A, Kimura T, Takenaka O, Iwamoto M (eds) Primatology today. Elsevier, Amsterdam New York, pp 81–84Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Anthropological InstituteUniversity Zürich-IrchelZürichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations