Advertisement

Primates

, Volume 47, Issue 3, pp 210–217 | Cite as

Social structures in Pan paniscus: testing the female bonding hypothesis

  • Jeroen M. G. Stevens
  • Hilde Vervaecke
  • Han De Vries
  • Linda Van Elsacker
Original Article

Abstract

Based on previous research in captivity, bonobos, Pan paniscus, have been called a female-bonded species. However, genetic and behavioural data indicate that wild females migrate. Bonding between these unrelated females would then be in contradiction with socio-ecological models. It has been argued that female bonding has been overemphasized in captive bonobos. We examine patterns of proximity, grooming and support behaviour in six well established captive groups of bonobos. We find that female bonding was not a typical characteristic of all captive bonobo groups. In only two groups there was a trend for females to prefer proximity with other females over association with males. We found no evidence that following or grooming between females was more frequent than between males and unrelated females or between males. Only in coalitions, females supported each other more than male–female or male–male dyads. We also investigated five mother–son pairs. Grooming was more frequent among mothers and sons than in any other dyad, but sons did not groom their mothers more than males groomed unrelated females. Mothers groomed their sons, or provided more support to them than females groomed or supported unrelated males. Thus, while bonds between females were clearly present, intersexual relations between males and either unrelated females or their mothers are of more, or equal importance.

Keywords

Captivity Coalitions Grooming Pan paniscus Social bonding 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the directory and keepers of Planckendael Wild Animal Park (Belgium), Apenheul Primate Park (The Netherlands), Wuppertal Zoo (Germany) and Twycross Zoo (UK) for their help and interest in this study. This research was funded by a Ph.D. grant of the Institution for the Promotion of Innovation through Science and Technology in Flanders (IWT-Vlaanderen: grant number 3340). We thank the Flemish Government for structural support of the Centre for Research and Conservation of the Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp. We thank three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Badrian A, Badrian N (1984) Social organization of Pan paniscus in the Lomako Forest, Zaire. In: Susman RL (ed) The pygmy chimpanzee: evolutionary ecology and behavior. Plenum Press, New York, pp 325–346Google Scholar
  3. Baker KC, Smuts BB (1994) Social relationships of female chimpanzees: diversity between captive social groups. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 227–242Google Scholar
  4. Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (2002) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Fruth B, Hohmann G, McGrew WC (1999) The Pan species. In: Dolhinow P, Fuentes A (eds) The nonhuman primates. Mayfield, London, pp 64–72Google Scholar
  6. Furuichi T (1989) Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. Int J Primatol 10:173–197Google Scholar
  7. Furuichi T (1997) Agonistic interactions and matrifocal dominance rank of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. Folia Primatol 18:855–875Google Scholar
  8. Furuichi T, Ihobe H (1994) Variation in male relationships in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour 130:212–228Google Scholar
  9. Furuichi T, Idani G, Ihobe H, Kuroda S, Kitamura K, Mori A, Enomoto T, Okayasu N, Hashimoto C, Kano T (1998) Population dynamics of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. Int J Primatol 19:1029–1043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gerloff U, Hartung B, Fruth B, Hohmann G, Tautz D (1999) Intracommunity relationships, dispersal pattern and paternity success in a wild living community of Bonobos (Pan paniscus) determined from DNA analysis of faecal samples. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:1189–1195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hashimoto C, Furuichi T, Takenaka O (1996) Matrilineal kin relationship and social behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus): sequencing the D-loop region of mitochondrial DNA. Primates 37:305–318Google Scholar
  12. Hemelrijk CK, Ek A (1991) Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and ‘support’ in captive chimpanzees. Anim Behav 41:923–935Google Scholar
  13. Hohmann G, Fruth B (2002) Dynamics in social organization of bonobos (Pan paniscus). In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 138–150Google Scholar
  14. Hohmann G, Fruth B (2003) Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. Curr Anthropol 44:563–571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hohmann G, Gerloff U, Fruth B (1999) Social bonds and genetic tests: kinship, association and affiliation in a community of bonobos (Pan paniscus). Behaviour 136:1219–1235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Idani G (1991) Social relationships between immigrant and resident bonobo (Pan paniscus) females at Wamba. Folia Primatol 57:83–95PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Ihobe H (1992) Male–male relationships among wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. Primates 33:163–179Google Scholar
  18. Kano T (1982) The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba. Primates 23:171–188Google Scholar
  19. Kano T (1992) The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford University Press, StanfordGoogle Scholar
  20. Kuroda S (1979) Grouping of pygmy chimpanzees. Primates 20:161–183Google Scholar
  21. Kuroda S (1980) Social behavior of pygmy chimpanzees. Primates 21:181–197Google Scholar
  22. Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  23. Muroyama Y, Sugiyama Y (1994) Grooming relationships in two species of chimpanzees. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 169–180Google Scholar
  24. Parish AR (1994) Sex and food control in the “uncommon chimpanzee”: how bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of male dominance. Ethol Sociobiol 15:157–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Parish AR (1996) Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Hum Nat 7:61–96Google Scholar
  26. Parish AR, de Waal FBM (2000) The other “closest living relative”: how bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution. Ann NY Acad Sci 907:97–113PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Strushaker TT (1987) Primate societies. The University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  28. Stanford CB (1998) The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. Curr Anthropol 39:399–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP (1997) The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:291–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stevens J, Vervaecke H, Melens W, Huyghe M, De Ridder P, Van Elsacker L (2003) Much ado about bonobos: ten years of management and research at Planckendael Wild Animal Park, Belgium. In: Gilbert T C (ed) Proceedings of the fifth annual symposium on zoo research, 7–8 July 2003, Marwell. Marwell Zoological Park, pp 114–125Google Scholar
  31. van Schaik CP (1989) The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Standen V, Foley RA (eds) Comparative socioecology: the behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 195–218Google Scholar
  32. Vervaecke H, De Vries H, Van Elsacker L (2000a) The pivotal role of rank in grooming and support behaviour in a captive group of bonobos (Pan paniscus). Behaviour 137:1463–1485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Vervaecke H, de Vries H, Van Elsacker L. (2000b) Function and distribution of coalitions in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Primates 41:249–265Google Scholar
  34. Vervaecke H, Stevens J, Van Elsacker L (2003) Interfering with others: female–female reproductive competition in Pan paniscus. In: Jones CB (ed) Sexual selection and reproductive competition in primates: new perspectives and directions. American Society of Primatologists, Oklahoma, pp 1235–1246Google Scholar
  35. de Waal FBM (1978) Exploitative and familiarity dependent support strategies in a colony of semi-free living chimpanzees. Behaviour 66:268–312Google Scholar
  36. de Waal FBM (1994) Chimpanzee’s adaptive potential. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 243–260Google Scholar
  37. de Waal FBM (1995) Bonobo sex and society. Sci Am 272(3):58–64Google Scholar
  38. Franz C (1999) Allogrooming behavior and grooming site preferences in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): association with female dominance. Int J Primatol 20:525–546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. White FJ (1988) Party composition and dynamics in Pan paniscus. Int J Primatol 9:179–193Google Scholar
  40. White FJ (1991) Pygmy chimpanzee social organization: variation with party size and between study sites. Am J Primatol 25:203–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wrangham RW (1980) An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–299Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre and Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeroen M. G. Stevens
    • 1
    • 2
  • Hilde Vervaecke
    • 1
    • 2
  • Han De Vries
    • 3
  • Linda Van Elsacker
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of AntwerpWilrijkBelgium
  2. 2.Centre for Research and ConservationRoyal Zoological Society of AntwerpAntwerpBelgium
  3. 3.Department of Behavioral BiologyUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations