Advertisement

European Journal of Psychology of Education

, Volume 29, Issue 1, pp 117–137 | Cite as

Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: a longitudinal study from a developmental perspective

  • Manita van der StelEmail author
  • Marcel V. J. Veenman
Article

Abstract

In the last decades, students increasingly have been placed in the role of active learners with responsibilities for their own learning. Students have to be able to plan their learning activities and execute them in a systematic and orderly way and to monitor and to evaluate their learning and to reflect on it. All aforementioned skills are components of metacognitive skillfulness. The first objective was to gain insight in the development of both quantity and quality of metacognitive skills in young adolescents (aged 12–15 years). The second objective was to establish whether development of metacognitive skills is intelligence related or relatively intelligence independent. Finally, the generality vs. domain specificity of developing metacognitive skills was investigated. In a 3-year longitudinal study, participants performed two different tasks (text studying and problem solving) in two different domains (history and math), while thinking aloud. Results show that between the age of 12 and 15 years, metacognitive skills do not develop linearly or at the same pace. Furthermore, metacognitive skills contribute to learning performance, partly independent of intellectual ability. Finally, the results show that metacognitive skills appear to be predominantly general by nature over the years. Although a smaller domain-specific component was found as well in the first 2 years, this component disintegrated in the third year. The age around 15 years appears to be a relevant point in time during the developmental trajectory of metacognitive skills: Growth is (temporarily) put on hold, while the nature of these skills becomes fully general.

Keywords

Metacognition Metacognitive skills Intellectual ability Development Adolescents 

Notes

References

  1. Alexander, J., Carr, M., & Schwanenflugel, M. (1995). Development of metacognition in gifted children: directions for future research. Developmental Review, 15, 1–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R. (1996). The architecture of cognition. Mohwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Annevirta, T., & Vauras, M. (2006). Developmental changes of metacognitive skill in elementary school children. The Journal of Experimental Education, 74, 197–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barnett, J. E. (2000). Self-regulated reading and test preparation among college students. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 31, 42–53.Google Scholar
  5. Blöte, A. W., Otterloo, S. G., Van Stevenson, C. E., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2004). Discovery and maintenance of the many-to-one counting strategy in 4-year-olds: a microgenetic study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 83–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Borkowski, J. G., & Peck, V. A. (1986). Causes and consequences of metamemory in gifted children. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Knowing, learning, and instruction. Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Bowen, S., Shore, B. M., & Cartwright, G. F. (1992). Do gifted children use computers differently? A view from “the factory”. Gifted Educational International, 8, 151–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, A. L. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In R. J. Spiro, B. Bruce, & W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Brown, A. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In R. S. Siegel (Ed.), Children’s thinking: what develops? Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Brown, A. L., Brandsford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed). Cognitive development (Vol. 3, pp. 515–529). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  11. Buskop, H., Dalhuisen, L., & Van Der Geest, R. (1998). Sprekend verleden, deel 2. Baarn: NijghVersluys.Google Scholar
  12. Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 62–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Christoph, N. L. H. (2006). The role of metacognitive skills in learning to solve problems. Wageningen: Prinsen & Looijen.Google Scholar
  15. Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S., Van Leijenhorst, L., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 9315–9320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. De Jong, F. P. C. M. (1992). Zelfstandig leren. Regulatie van het leerproces en leren reguleren: een procesbenadering. Tilburg: Katholieke Universiteit.Google Scholar
  17. Demetriou, A., & Efklides, A. (1990). The objective and subjective structure of problem-solving abilities: metacognitive awareness from early adolescence to middle age. In H. Mandl, E. de Corte, S. N. Bennett, & H. F. Friedrich (Eds.), Learning and instruction in an international context: volume 2.1. Social and cognitive aspects of learning and instruction (pp. 161–179). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  18. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective taking. In H. Beilin & P. Pufall (Eds.), Piaget’s theory: prospects and possibilities (pp. 107–141). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. Glaser, R., Schauble, L., Raghavan, K., & Zeitz, C. (1992). Scientific reasoning across different domains. In E. de Corte, M. C. Linn, H. Mandl, & L. Verschaffel (Eds.), Computer-based learning environments and problem solving. NATO ASI series F, vol. 84 (pp. 345–371). Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Guilford, J. P. (1965). Fundamental statistics in psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  22. Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. London: Routledge and Kegan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kelemen, W. L., Frost, P. J., & Weaver, C. A., III. (2000). Individual differences in metacognition: evidence against a general metacognitive ability. Memory and Cognition, 28, 92–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kinnunen, R., & Vauras, M. (1995). Comprehension monitoring and the level of comprehension in high- and low-achieving primary school childrens’ reading. Learning and Instruction, 5, 143–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kuhn, D. (1999). Metacognitive development. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-Lemonda (Eds.), Child psychology. A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 259–286). Philadelphia: Psychology.Google Scholar
  26. Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 178–181.Google Scholar
  27. Larkin, S. (2006). Collaborative group work and individual development of metacognition in early years. Research in Science Education, 36, 7–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2006). Precursors of metamemory in young children: the role of theory of mind and metacognitive vocabulary. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 15–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Markman, E. (1977). Realizing that you don’t understand: a preliminary investigation. Child Development, 48, 986–992.Google Scholar
  30. Markman, E. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Meijer, J., Veenman, M. V. J., & Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. (2006). Metacognitive activities in text studying and problem solving: development of a taxonomy. Educational Research and Evaluation, 3, 209–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mevarech, Z., & Fridkin, S. (2006). The effects of IMPROVE on mathematical knowledge, mathematical reasoning and metacognition. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 85–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Nickerson, R., Perkins, D., & Smith, E. (1985). The teaching of thinking. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  34. Nunnally, J. C. (1967). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  35. Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  36. Pressley, M. (2000). Development of grounded theories of complex cognitive processing: exhaustive within- and between-study analyses of think-aloud data. In G. Schraw & J. C. Impara (Eds.), Issues in the measurement of metacognition. Lincoln: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.Google Scholar
  37. Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: the nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Pressley, M., Yokoi, L., Van Meter, P., Van Etten, S., & Freebern, G. (1997). Some of the reasons why preparing for an exam is so hard: what can be done to make it easier? Educational Psychology Review, 9, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Prins, F. J., Veenman, M. V. J., & Elshout, J. J. (2006). The impact of intellectual ability and metacognition on learning: new support for the threshold of problematicity theory. Learning and Instruction, 4, 374–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Schneider, W. (1985). Developmental trends in the meta-memory memory behavior relationship: An integrative review. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. Mac Kinnon, & T. G. Wallers (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition and human performance (Vol. 1, pp. 57–109). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  42. Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  43. Schoenfeld, A. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook for research on mathematics and learning (pp. 334–370). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  44. Schraw, G., & Nietfeld, J. (1998). A further test of the general monitoring skill hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 236–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schraw, G., Dunkle, M. E., Bendixen, L. D., & Roedel, T. D. (1995). Does a general monitoring skill exist? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 433–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Shore, B. M., & Lazar, L. (1996). IQ-related differences in time allocation during problem solving. Psychological Reports, 78, 848–849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Siegler, R. S., DeLoache, J. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2010). How children develop (3rd ed.). New York: Worth.Google Scholar
  48. Slife, B. D., Weiss, J., & Bell, T. (1985). Separability of metacognition and cognition: problem solving in learning disabled and regular students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 437–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavior manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 69–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of the mind: conceptions of the nature of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Thorpe, K. J., & Satterly, D. J. H. (1990). The development and inter-relationship of metacognitive components among primary school children. Educational Psychology, 10, 5–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 148–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Van Boxtel, C. A. M., & Schrover, W. (1998). MeMo: Geschiedenis voor de basisvorming, deel 2, Mavo/Havo/Vwo. Den Bosch: Malmberg.Google Scholar
  55. Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2008). Relation between intellectual ability and metacognitive skillfulness as predictors of learning performance of young students performing tasks in different domains. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 128–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2010). Development of metacognitive skillfulness: a longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 220–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Van der Stel, M., Veenman, M. V. J., Deelen, K., & Haenen, J. (2010). Increasing role of metacognitive skills in math: a cross-sectional study from a developmental perspective. ZDM International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42, 219–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Van Dijk, H., & Tellegen, P. J. (1994). GIVO. Groninger Intelligentietest Voortgezet Onderwijs. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
  59. Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. (1986). Markeren van kerngedeelten in studieteksten. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
  60. Veenman, M. V. J. (1993). Intellectual ability and metacognitive skill: determinants of discovery learning in computerized environments. Dissertation, Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  61. Veenman, M. V. J. (2011). Learning to self-monitor and self-regulate. In R. Mayer & P. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 197–218). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  62. Veenman, M. V. J., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). Intellectual and metacognitive skills of novices while studying texts under conditions of text difficulty and time constraint. Learning and Instruction, 14, 621–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Veenman, M. V. J., & Spaans, M. A. (2005). Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills: age and task differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 159–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Veenman, M. V. J., & Verheij, J. (2003). Technical students’ metacognitive skills: relating general vs. specific metacognitive skills to study success. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 259–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Veenman, M. V. J., Elshout, J. J., & Meijer, J. (1997). The generality vs. domain-specifity of metacognitive skills in novice learning across domains. Learning and Instruction, 7, 187–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Veenman, M. V. J., Kerseboom, L., & Imthorn, C. (2000). Test anxiety and metacognitive skillfulness: availability versus production deficiencies. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 13, 391–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14, 89–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Veenman, M. V. J., Bavelaar, L., De Wolf, L., & Van Haaren, M. G. P. (2013). The on-line assessment of metacognitive skills in a computerized learning environment. Learning and Individual Differences. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.003.Google Scholar
  70. Vuijk, R., et al. (2003). Getal en Ruimte. Houten: EPN.Google Scholar
  71. Vukman, K. B., & Licardo, M. (2010). How cognitive, metacognitive, motivational and emotional self-regulation influence school performance in adolescence and early adulthood. Educational Studies, 36, 259–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1990). What influences learning? A content analysis of review literature. The Journal of Educational Research, 84, 30–43.Google Scholar
  73. Wellman, H. M. (1990). Children’s theories of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  74. Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Pino Pasternak, D., Sangster, C., Grau, V., Bingham, S., et al. (2009). The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children. Metacognition and Learning, 4, 63–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Winne, P. H. (1996). A metacognitive view of individual differences in self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 327–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Winne, P. H. (2010). Improving measurements of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 45, 267–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada, Lisboa, Portugal and Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Developmental and Educational PsychologyLeiden UniversityLeidenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations