European Journal of Psychology of Education

, Volume 28, Issue 3, pp 879–902 | Cite as

Justification beliefs and multiple-documents comprehension

  • Ivar Bråten
  • Leila E. Ferguson
  • Helge I. Strømsø
  • Øistein Anmarkrud
Article

Abstract

Building on the multidimensional framework of epistemic cognition proposed by Greene et al. (Educational Psychologist 43:142–160, 2008), this study examined beliefs about justification of knowledge claims in science among 65 Norwegian 10th graders. The first research question asked whether beliefs in personal justification, justification by authority, and justification by multiple sources differed in strength among the participants. It was found that the students most strongly believed in justification by authority, followed by justification by multiple sources and personal justification. The second research question asked whether the three types of justification beliefs differentially and uniquely predicted the comprehension of multiple conflicting documents on a science issue. In a multiple regression analysis with multiple-documents comprehension indicated by essay performance as the dependent variable, both personal justification and justification by multiple sources emerged as unique predictors when topic knowledge was controlled for. Specifically, beliefs in personal opinion as a means of justifying knowledge claims in science was negatively related to multiple-documents comprehension, whereas beliefs in justification through corroboration across multiple sources of information were positively related to multiple-documents comprehension. This study provides new evidence about relationships between epistemic beliefs and new literacy competencies needed in an information society, such as integrating across multiple conflicting sources of information; relationships that may also have practical implications.

Keywords

Epistemic beliefs Justification for knowing Multiple-documents comprehension Adolescent readers 

References

  1. Afflerbach, P., & Cho, B.-Y. (2009). Identifying and describing constructively responsive comprehension strategies in new and traditional forms of reading. In S. E. Israel & G. G. Duffy (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (pp. 69–90). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  2. Alexander, P. A., & Fox, E. (2011). Adolescents as readers. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, vol. 4 (pp. 156–176). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson, R. C. (2004). Role of the reader’s schema in comprehension, learning, and memory. In R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (3rd ed., pp. 594–606). Newark: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
  4. Björnsson, C. H. (1968). Läsbarhet [Readability]. Stockholm: Liber.Google Scholar
  5. Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2006). Effects of personal epistemology on the understanding of multiple texts. Reading Psychology, 27, 457–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010a). Effects of task instruction and personal epistemology on the understanding of multiple texts about climate change. Discourse Processes, 47, 1–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010b). When law students read multiple documents about global warming: examining the role of topic-specific beliefs about the nature of knowledge and knowing. Instructional Science, 38, 635–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2011). Measuring strategic processing when students read multiple texts. Metacognition and Learning, 6, 111–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2008). Are sophisticated students always better? The role of topic-specific personal epistemology in the understanding of multiple expository texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 814–840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bråten, I., Gil, L., Strømsø, H. I., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2009a). Personal epistemology across cultures: exploring Norwegian and Spanish university students’ epistemic beliefs about climate change. Social Psychology of Education, 12, 529–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Britt, M. A. (2009b). Trust matters: examining the role of source evaluation in students’ construction of meaning within and across multiple texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 6–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bråten, I., Britt, M. A., Strømsø, H. I., & Rouet, J. F. (2011a). The role of epistemic beliefs in the comprehension of multiple expository texts: toward an integrated model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 48–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bråten, I., Strømsø, H. I., & Salmerón, L. (2011b). Trust and mistrust when students read multiple information sources about climate change. Learning and Instruction, 21, 180–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bråten, I., Ferguson, L. E., Strømsø, H. I., & Anmarkrud, Ø. (2012). Students working with multiple conflicting documents on a science issue: Relations between epistemic cognition while reading and sourcing and argumentation in essays (submitted).Google Scholar
  15. Britt, M. A., & Aglinskas, C. (2002). Improving students’ ability to identify and use source information. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 485–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Britt, M. A., & Rouet, J. F. (2012). Learning with multiple documents: component skills and their acquisition. In J. R. Kirby & M. J. Lawson (Eds.), Enhancing the quality of learning: Dispositions, instruction, and learning processes (pp. 276–314). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Britt, M. A., Perfetti, C. A., Sandak, R., & Rouet, J. F. (1999). Content integration and source separation in learning from multiple texts. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative, comprehension, causality, and coherence: essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 209–233). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Bromme, R., Kienhues, D., & Stahl, E. (2008). Knowledge and epistemological beliefs: an intimate but complicate relationship. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs: epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 423–443). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Bromme, R., Kienhues, D., & Porsch, T. (2010). Who knows what and who can we believe? Epistemic beliefs are beliefs about knowledge (mostly) attained from others. In L. D. Bendixen & F. C. Feucht (Eds.), Personal epistemology in the classroom: theory, research, and implications for practice (pp. 163–193). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Buehl, M. M. (2008). Assessing the multidimensionality of students’ epistemic beliefs across diverse cultures. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs: epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 65–112). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Buehl, M. M., & Alexander, P. A. (2001). Beliefs about academic knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 385–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Buehl, M. M., & Alexander, P. A. (2005). Motivation and performance differences in students’ domain-specific epistemological belief profiles. American Educational Research Journal, 42, 697–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Chinn, C.A., & Buckland, L.A. (2012, April). A multiparameter framework for the design of assessments of epistemic cognition. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Vancouver, Canada.Google Scholar
  25. Chinn, C. A., Buckland, L. A., & Samarapungavan, A. (2011). Expanding the dimensions of epistemic cognition: arguments from philosophy and psychology. Educational Psychologist, 46, 141–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Coté, N. C., Goldman, S. R., & Saul, E. U. (1998). Students making sense of informational text: relations between processing and representation. Discourse Processes, 25, 1–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ferguson, L. E., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2012a). Epistemic cognition when students read multiple documents containing conflicting scientific evidence: a think-aloud study. Learning and Instruction, 22, 103–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ferguson, L. E., Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I., & Anmarkrud, Ø. (2012b). Dimensionality and change in epistemic beliefs when adolescents read conflicting information presented in multiple documents (submitted)Google Scholar
  30. Fox, E. (2009). The role of reader characteristics in processing and learning from informational text. Review of Educational Research, 79, 197–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gil, L., Bråten, I., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Strømsø, H. I. (2010). Summary versus argument tasks when working with multiple documents: Which is better for whom? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 157–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Goldman, S. R. (2004). Cognitive aspects of constructing meaning through and across multiple texts. In N. Shuart-Faris & D. Bloome (Eds.), Uses of intertextuality in classroom and educational research (pp. 317–351). Greenwich: Information Age.Google Scholar
  33. Goldman, S. R., Ozuru, Y., Braasch, J. L. G., Manning, F. H., Lawless, K. A., Gomez, K. W., & Slanovits, M. J. (2011). Literacies for learning: a multiple source comprehension illustration. In N. L. Stein & S. W. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental cognitive science goes to school (pp. 30–44). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  34. Greene, J. A., Azevedo, R., & Torney-Purta, J. (2008). Modeling epistemic and ontological cognition: philosophical perspectives and methodological directions. Educational Psychologist, 43, 142–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Greene, J. A., Torney-Purta, J., & Azevedo, R. (2010). Empirical evidence regarding relations among a model of epistemic and ontological cognition, academic performance, and educational level. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 234–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hofer, B. K. (2004). Exploring the dimensions of personal epistemology in differing classroom contexts: student interpretations during the first year of college. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 129–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jacobson, M. J., & Spiro, R. J. (1995). Hypertext learning environments, cognitive flexibility, and the transfer of complex knowledge: an empirical investigation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 12, 301–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kardash, C. M., & Howell, K. L. (2000). Effects of epistemological beliefs and topic-specific beliefs on undergraduates’ cognitive and strategic processing of dual-positional text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 524–535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kardash, C. M., & Scholes, R. J. (1996). Effects of preexisting beliefs, epistemological beliefs, and need for cognition on interpretation of controversial issues. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 260–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kienhues, D., Bromme, R., & Stahl, E. (2008). Changing epistemological beliefs: the unexpected impact of short-term intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 545–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kienhues, D., Stadtler, M., & Bromme, R. (2011). Dealing with conflicting or consistent medical information on the web: when expert information breeds laypersons’ doubts about experts. Learning and Instruction, 21, 193–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: a paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Kobayashi, K. (2009). The influence of topic knowledge, external strategy use, and college experience on students’ comprehension of controversial texts. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 130–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O’Loughlin, M. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. Orlando: Academic.Google Scholar
  46. Le Bigot, L., & Rouet, J. F. (2007). The impact of presentation format, task assignment, and prior knowledge on students’ comprehension of multiple online documents. Journal of Literacy Research, 39, 445–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Limon, M. (2006). The domain generality-specificity of epistemological beliefs: a theoretical problem, a methodological problem, or both? International Journal of Educational Research, 45, 7–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mason, L., Scirica, F., & Salvi, L. (2006). Effects of beliefs about meaning construction and task instructions on interpretation of narrative text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 411–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mason, L., Boldrin, A., & Ariasi, N. (2010a). Epistemic metacognition in context: evaluating and learning online information. Metacognition and Learning, 4, 67–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mason, L., Boldrin, A., & Ariasi, N. (2010b). Searching the web to learn about a controversial topic: are students epistemically active? Instructional Science, 38, 607–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mason, L., Ariasi, N., & Boldrin, A. (2011). Epistemic beliefs in action: spontaneous reflections about knowledge and knowing during online information searching and their influences on learning. Learning and Instruction, 21, 137–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Metzger, M. J., & Flanagin, A. J. (Eds.). (2008). Digital media, youth, and credibility. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  53. Miles, J. N. V., & Shevlin, M. (2001). Applying regression and correlation: a guide for students and researchers. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  54. Moos, D. C., & Azevedo, R. (2008). Self-regulated learning with hypermedia: the role of prior domain knowledge. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 270–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Mugalogu, E. Z., & Bayram, H. (2010). A structural model of prospective science teachers’ nature of science views. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 54, 597–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Muis, K. R., Bendixen, L. D., & Haerle, F. C. (2006). Domain-generality and domain-specificity in personal epistemology research: philosophical and empirical questions in the development of a theoretical model. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 3–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Murphy, P. K., & Alexander, P. A. (2004). Persuasion as a dynamic, multidimensional process: an investigation of individual and intraindividual differences. American Educational Research Journal, 41, 337–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Murphy, P. K., & Mason, L. (2006). Changing knowledge and beliefs. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 305–324). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  59. Norwegian Ministry of Education and Research. (2006). Læreplan for grunnskolen og videregående skole [Curriculum for elementary and secondary school]. Oslo: Ministry of Education and Research.Google Scholar
  60. Paxton, R. J. (2002). The influence of author visibility on high school students solving a historical problem. Cognition and Instruction, 20, 197–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Perfetti, C. A., Rouet, J. F., & Britt, M. A. (1999). Toward a theory of documents representation. In H. Van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representation during reading (pp. 99–122). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  62. Pieschl, S., Stahl, E., & Bromme, R. (2008). Epistemological beliefs and self-regulated learning with hypertext. Metacognition and Learning, 3, 17–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Rouet, J. F. (2006). The skills of document use: from text comprehension to web-based learning. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  64. Rouet, J. F., Britt, M. A., Mason, R. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using multiple sources of evidence to reason about history. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 478–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rouet, J. F., Favart, M., Britt, M. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Studying and using multiple documents in history: effects of discipline expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 85–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rukavina, I., & Daneman, M. (1996). Integration and its effect on acquiring knowledge about competing scientific theories from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 272–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schommer, M., & Dunnell, P. A. (1997). Epistemological beliefs and gifted high school students. Roeper Review, 19, 153–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Schommer, M., & Walker, K. (1995). Are epistemological beliefs similar across domains? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 424–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Schommer, M., Crouse, A., & Rhodes, N. (1992). Epistemological beliefs and mathematical text comprehension: believing it is simple does not make it so. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 435–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Schraw, G. (2000). Reader beliefs and meaning construction in narrative text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 96–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Schraw, G., Bendixen, L. D., & Dunkle, M. E. (2002). Development and validation of the Epistemic Belief Inventory (EBI). In B. K. Hofer & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: the psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 261–275). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  73. Spiro, R. J., Feltovich, P. J., Jacobson, M. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1991). Cognitive flexibility, constructivism, and hypertext: random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. Educational Technology, 31(5), 24–33.Google Scholar
  74. Spiro, R. J., Coulson, R. L., Feltovich, P. J., & Anderson, D. K. (1994). Cognitive flexibility theory: advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. In R. B. Ruddell, M. R. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 602–615). Newark: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
  75. Stahl, E., & Bromme, R. (2007). The CAEB: an instrument for measuring connotative aspects of epistemological beliefs. Learning and Instruction, 17, 773–785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Stahl, S. A., Hynd, C. R., Britton, B. K., McNish, M. M., & Bosquet, D. (1996). What happens when students read multiple source documents in history? Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 430–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Strømsø, H. I., & Bråten, I. (2009). Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and multiple-text comprehension among upper secondary students. Educational Psychology, 29, 425–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2003). Students’ strategic use of multiple sources during expository text reading. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 113–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2008). Dimensions of topic-specific epistemological beliefs as predictors of multiple text understanding. Learning and Instruction, 18, 513–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  81. Thomm, E., & Bromme, R. (2012). “It should at least seem scientific!” Textual features of “scientificness” and their impact on lay assessments of online information. Science Education, 96, 187–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Trautwein, U., & Lüdtke, O. (2007). Predicting global and topic-specific certainty beliefs: domain-specificity and the role of the academic environment. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 907–934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Tsai, C. C., Ho, H. N. J., Liang, J. C., & Lin, H. M. (2011). Scientific epistemic beliefs, conceptions of learning science and self-efficacy of learning science among high school students. Learning and Instruction, 21, 757–769.Google Scholar
  84. VanSledright, B., & Kelly, C. (1998). Reading American history: the influence on multiple sources on six fifth graders. The Elementary School Journal, 98, 239–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Vidal-Abarca, E., Martínez, T., Salmerón, L., Cerdán, R., Gilabert, R., Gil, L., Mañá, A., Llorens, A., & Ferris, R. (2011). Recording online processes in task-oriented reading with Read&Answer. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 179–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Vinje, F. E. (1982). Journalistspråket [The journalist language]. Fredrikstad: Institute for Journalism.Google Scholar
  87. Wiley, J., & Voss, J. F. (1996). The effects of “playing” historian on learning in history. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Wiley, J., & Voss, J. F. (1999). Constructing arguments from multiple sources: tasks that promote understanding and not just memory for text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 301–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Wineburg, S. (1991). Historical problem solving: a study of the cognitive processes used in the evaluation of documentary and pictorial evidence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Wolfe, M. B. W., & Goldman, S. R. (2005). Relations between adolescents’ text processing and reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 23, 467–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada, Lisboa, Portugal and Springer Science+Business Media BV 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ivar Bråten
    • 1
  • Leila E. Ferguson
    • 1
  • Helge I. Strømsø
    • 1
  • Øistein Anmarkrud
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Educational ResearchUniversity of OsloOsloNorway

Personalised recommendations