Journal of Ethology

, Volume 29, Issue 2, pp 389–393 | Cite as

Sharing the moment: the duration of embraces in humans

Short Communication

Abstract

Moments of some gestures may stand out from our constantly flowing life-time as memorable experiences. We feel subjective moments as our psychological present. Few behavioral acts can express a shared subjective experience so easily observable than people’s embraces after a significant life-event. Spontaneous embraces from the 2008 Summer Olympics Games were analysed, and were found to last for about 3 s, comparable to previously described perceptual and motor units in humans and also in other primate species. These 3-s segments of time are suggested to be the basic temporal building blocks of our behaviourally expressed subjective experiences.

Keywords

Movement patterns Intersubjectivity Event durations Humans Phenomenology 

References

  1. Bierman DJ, Radin DI (1997) Anomalous anticipatory response on randomized future conditions. Percept Mot Skills 84:689–690PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Chafe W (1987) Cognitive constraints on information flow. In: Tomlin RS (ed) Coherence and grounding in discourse. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 21–51Google Scholar
  3. Efron R (2006) The duration of the present. Ann N Y Acad Sci 138:713–729CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Feldthutter I, Schleidt M, Eibl-Eibesfeldt I (1990) Moving in the beat of seconds: analysis of the time structure of human action. Ethol Sociobiol 11:511–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Fraisse P (1984) Perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev Psychol 35:1–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gerstner GE, Goldberg LJ (1994) Evidence of a time constant associated with movement patterns in six mammalian species. Ethol Sociobiol 15:181–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Kien J, Schleidt M, Schottner B (1991) Temporal segmentation in hand movements of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and comparisons with humans. Ethology 89:297–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Martin JG (1972) Rhythmic (hierarchical) versus serial structure in speech and other behavior. Psychol Rev 79:487–509PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Mates J, Müller U, Radil T, Pöppel E (1994) Temporal integration in sensorimotor synchronization. J Cog Neurosci 6:332–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Matthews D, Fox E (1976) The physiological basis of physical education and athletics, 2nd edn. Saunders, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  11. Miyake Y, Heiss J, Pöppel E (2001) Dual anticipation in sensori-motor synchronization. In: Proceedings of the First international symposium on measurement, analysis and modeling of human functions, Sapporo, pp 61–66Google Scholar
  12. Noldus Information Technology (2003) The ObserverR reference manual, version 5.0. Wageningen, The NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
  13. Parncutt R, Pascall R (2002) Middle-out music analysis and its psychological basis. In: Stevens C, Burnham D, McPherson G, Schubert E, Renwick J (eds) Proceedings of the 7th international conference on music perception and cognition casual productions, AdelaideGoogle Scholar
  14. Pöppel E (1978) Time perception. In: Held R, Leibowitz HW, Teuber HL (eds) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol 8. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 713–729Google Scholar
  15. Pöppel E (2004) Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 64:295–301Google Scholar
  16. Sams M, Hari R, Rig J, Knuutila J (1993) The human auditory sensory memory trace persists about 10 sec: neuromagnetic evidence. J Cog Neurosci 5:363–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Schleidt M (1988) Universal time constant operating in human short-term behavior repetitions. Ethology 77:67–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Schleidt M, Kien J (1997) Segmentation in behavior and what it can tell us about brain function. Hum Nat 8:77–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Stern DN (2004) The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Tortoram GJ, Anagnostakos NP (1990) Principles of anatomy and physiology, 6th edn. Harper-Collins, New York, p 707Google Scholar
  21. Trevarthen C (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse. Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Music Sci Special Issue 155–215Google Scholar
  22. Turner F, Pöppel E (1983) The natural lyre: poetic meter, the brain and time. Poetry 277–309Google Scholar
  23. Vollrath M, Kazenwadel J, Krüger HP (1992) A universal constant in temporal segmentation of human speech. Naturwissenschaften 79:479–480PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Wang SC, Borison HL (1947) An analysis of the carotid sinus cardiovascular reflex mechanism. Am J Physiol 150:712–728PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Warner HR, Cox A (1962) A mathematical model of heart rate control by sympathetic and vagal efferent information. J Appl Physiol 17:349–355PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Warner HR, Russell RO (1969) Effect of combined sympathetic and vagal stimulation on heart rate in the dog. Circ Res 24:567–573PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Wundt W (1911) Introduction into psychology (in German). Voigtländer, LeipzigGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Ethological Society and Springer 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of DundeeDundeeUK

Personalised recommendations