Journal of Ethology

, Volume 25, Issue 3, pp 271–279

Observational visuospatial encoding of the cache locations of others by western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica)

Article

Abstract

Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) hide food and rely on spatial memory to recover their caches at a later date. They also rely on observational spatial memory to steal caches made by other individuals. Successful pilfering may require an understanding of allocentric space because the observer will often be in a different position from the demonstrator when the caching event occurs. We compared cache recovery accuracy of pairs of observers that watched a demonstrator cache food. The pattern of recovery searches showed that observers were more accurate when they had observed the caching event from the same viewing direction as the demonstrator than when they had watched from the opposite direction. Search accuracy was not affected by whether or not the tray-specific local cues provided left–right landmark information (i.e. heterogeneous vs. homogeneous local cues), or whether or not the caching tray location was rotated. Taken together, these results suggest that observers have excellent spatial memory and that they have little difficulty with mental rotation.

Keywords

Observational learning Mimic Visual perspective taking Mental rotation 

References

  1. Baron-Cohen S (1988) Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective? J Autism Dev Disord 18:379–402PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Balda RP, Kamil A (1992) Long-term spatial memory in Clark’s nutcracker. Nucifraga columbiana. Anim Behav 44:761–769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bednekoff PA, Balda RP (1996a) Social caching and observational spatial memory in pinyon jays. Behav 133:807–826Google Scholar
  4. Bednekoff PA, Balda RP (1996b) Observational spatial memory in Clark’s nutcrackers and Mexican jays. Anim Behav 52:833–839CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bednekoff PA, Balda RP (1997) Clark’s nutcracker spatial memory: Manay errors might not due to forgetting. Anim Behav 54:691–698PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brockmann HJ, Barnard CJ (1979) Kleptoparasitism in birds. Anim Behav 27:487–514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brodin A, Kunz C (1997) An experimental study of cache recovery by hoarding willow tits after different retention intervals. Behaviour 134:881–890Google Scholar
  8. Brodin A, Ekman J (1994) Benefits of food hoarding. Nature 372:510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2001) Do ravens manipulate the others´ attention in order to prevent or achieve social learning opportunities? Adv Ethol 36:106Google Scholar
  10. Burnell KL, Tomback DF (1985) Stellar’s jays steal gray jay caches: field and laboratory observations. Auk 102:417–419Google Scholar
  11. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395:272–278PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1999). Scrub Jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) remember the relative time of caching as well as the location and content of their caches. J Comp Psychol 113:403–416PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clayton NS, Griffiths DP, Emery NJ, Dickinson A (2001) Elements of episode-like memory in animals. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 356:1483–1491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coussi-Korbl S (1994) Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). J Com Psychol 108:164–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ekman J, Brodin A, Sklepkovych B (1995) Selfish long-term benefits of hoarding in the Siberian jay. Behav Ecol 7:140–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibb JA (1960) Populations of tits and gold crests and their food supply in pine populations. Ibis 102:163–208Google Scholar
  17. Healy SD, Suhonen J (1996) Memory for locations of stored food in willow tits and marsh tits. Behaviour 133:71–80Google Scholar
  18. Heinrich B, Pepper JW (1998) Influence of competitors on caching behavior in the common raven, Corvus corax. Anim Behav 56:1083–1090PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Heinrich B (1999) Mind of the Raven. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Held S, Mendl M, Devereux C, Byrne RW (2000) Social tactics of pigs in a competitive foraging task: the “informed forager” paradigm. Anim Behav 46:177–188Google Scholar
  21. Herman LM, Pack AA, Morrel-Samuels P (1993) Representational and conceptual skills of dolphins. In: Roitblatt HL, Herman LM, Nachtigall PE (Eds) Language and communication: comparative perspectives. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 403–442Google Scholar
  22. Hitchcock CL, Sherry DF (1990) Long-term memory for cache sites in the black-capped chickadee. Anim Behav 40:701–712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hollard VD, Delius JD (1982) Rotational invariance in visual pattern recognition by pigeons and humans. Science 218:804–806PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hopkins WD, Fagot J, Vauclair J (1993) Mirror-image matching and mental rotation problem solving by baboons (Papio papio): unilateral input enhances performance. J Exp Psychol: General 122:61–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Huttenlocher J, Presson CC (1973) Mental rotation and the perspective problem. Cogn Psychol 4:279–299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Huttenlocher J, Presson CC (1979) The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cogn Psychol 11:375–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mauck B, Dehnhardt G (1997) Mental rotation in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). J Exp Biol 200:1309–1316PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1956) The child7s conception of space. Routledge and Kegan Paul, LondonGoogle Scholar
  29. Presson CC (1982) Strategies in spatial reasoning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:3–11Google Scholar
  30. Preston S, Jacobs L (2005) Decisions in Merriam’s kangaroo rat (Dipodomys merriami). J Comp Psychol 119:187–196PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Shepard S, Metzler D (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Shettleworth SJ (1995) Memory in food-storing birds: From the field to the Skinner box. In: Alleva E, Fasolo A, Lipp H-P, Nadel L (eds) Proceedings of NATO Advanced Study Institute Series Maratea, Italy. Kluwer Academic Publishers, The Hague, pp 158–179Google Scholar
  33. Vader Wall SB (1982) An experimental analysis of cache recovery in Clark’s nutcracker. Anim Behav 30:84–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Watanabe S (1997) An instance of viewpoint consistency in pigeon object recognition. Behav Proc 39:257–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Watanabe S (1999) Enhancement of viewpoint invariance by experience in pigeons. Curr Psychol Cogn 18:322–336Google Scholar
  36. Watanabe S (2005) Strategies of spatial learning for food storing in scrub jays. J Ethiol 23:181–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Whiten A, Perner J (1991) Fundamental issues in the multidisciplinary study of mind reading. In: Whiten A (Ed) Natural theories of mind: evolution, development and simulation of everyday mind reading. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Ethological Society and Springer 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyKeio UniversityTokyoJapan
  2. 2.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations