Journal of Ethology

, Volume 24, Issue 3, pp 291–295 | Cite as

Female Iberian midwife toads (Alytes cisternasii) use call order to reach particular males in dense choruses

Article

Abstract

Call-timing mechanisms among anuran males calling within choruses are frequent, and previous studies have indicated that midwife toad females respond preferentially to these mechanisms. The results of multi-speaker tests performed here with midwife toads suggest that the temporal order of male calls within a dense chorus can determine the ability of females to locate the most attractive calls (in this instance the low-frequency calls emitted by larger males). When call emission was regular in the multi-speaker tests, most females chose the most attractive option (i.e., the speaker emitting the lower-frequency call). When call emission was not regular, however, most females failed to reach the most attractive option, selecting the speaker that emitted immediately after that emitting the most attractive call. These results support the idea that there may be benefits for smaller males that emit their less attractive calls close to those of larger, more attractive males in dense choruses. Less attractive males could exploit the attractiveness of nearby larger males calls by alternating their calls in an unordered sequence to reach receptive females.

Keywords

Call order effect Multi-speaker playback tests Female mate choice Iberian midwife toad Alytes cisternasii 

References

  1. Bosch J, Márquez R (2000) Acoustical interference in the advertisement calls of the midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and A. cisternasii). Behaviour 137:249–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bosch J, Márquez R (2001) Call timing in male–male acoustical interactions and female choice in the midwife toad Alytes obstetricans. Copeia 2001:169–177Google Scholar
  3. Bosch J, Márquez R (2002) Female preference function related to precedence effect in an amphibian anuran (Alytes cisternasii). Tests with non-overlapping calls. Behav Ecol 13:149–153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bosch J, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (2000) Signal variation and call preferences for the whine frequency in the túngara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:62–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bosch J, Márquez R, Boyero L (2003) Behavioural patterns, preference and motivation of female midwife toads during phonotaxis tests. J Ethol 21:61–66Google Scholar
  6. Gerhardt HC (1987) Evolutionary and neurobiological implications of selective phonotaxis in the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea. Anim Behav 35:1479–1489Google Scholar
  7. Gerhardt HC (1994) The evolution of vocalization in frogs and toads. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 25:293–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gerhardt HC, Schul J (1999) A quantitative analysis of behavioral selectivity for pulse rise-time in the gray treefrog. J Comp Physiol A 185:33–40CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Greenfield MD (1994) Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of signal interactions. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 25:97–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Greenfield MD, Tourtellot MK, Snedden WA (1997) Precedence effect and the evolution of chorusing. Proc R Soc Lon B 264:1335–1361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Halliday TR, Tejedo M (1995) Intrasexual selection and alternative mating behaviour. In: Heatwole H, Sullivan BK (eds) Amphibian biology, vol. 2, Social behaviour. Surrey Beatty, Sydney, pp 419–468Google Scholar
  12. Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev 72:283–327CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Klump GM, Gerhardt HC (1992) Mechanisms and function of call-timing in male–male interactions in frogs. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp 153–174Google Scholar
  14. Márquez R (1993) Male reproductive success in two midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and A. cisternasii). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 32:283–291Google Scholar
  15. Márquez R (1995) Female choice in the midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and A. cisternasii). Behaviour 132:151–161Google Scholar
  16. Márquez R, Bosch J (1995) Advertisement calls of the midwife toads Alytes (Amphibia, Anura, Discoglossidae) in continental Spain. J Zool Syst Evol Res 33:185–192Google Scholar
  17. Márquez R, Bosch J (1997) Female preference in complex acoustical environments in the midwife toads Alytes obstetricans and Alytes cisternasii. Behav Ecol 8:588–594Google Scholar
  18. Márquez R, Bosch J (2001) Communication and mating in the midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and Alytes cisternasii). In: Ryan MJ (ed) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, pp 220–231Google Scholar
  19. Márquez R, Verrell P (1991) The courtship and mating of the Iberian midwife toad, Alytes cisternasii (Amphibia, Anura, Discoglossidae). J Zool 225:125–139Google Scholar
  20. Ryan MJ (1991) Sexual selection and communication in frogs: some recent advances. Trends Ecol Evol 6:351–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ryan MJ (2001) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
  22. Schwartz JJ (1987) The function of call alternation in anuran amphibians: A test of three hypotheses. Evolution 41:461–471Google Scholar
  23. Telford SD, Dyson ML, Passmore NI (1989) Mate choice occurs only in small choruses of painted reed frogs Hyperolius marmoratus. Bioacoustics 2:47–53Google Scholar
  24. Wells KD (1977) The social behavior of anuran amphibians. Anim Behav 25:666–693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Wilczynski W, Rand AS, Ryan MJ (1999) Female preferences for temporal order of call components in the túngara frog: a Bayesian analysis. Anim Behav 58:841–851CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Zar JH (1996) Biostatistical analysis. Prentice Hall, New JerseyGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Ethological Society and Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales, CSICMadridSpain
  2. 2.School of Tropical BiologyJames Cook UniversityTownsvilleAustralia

Personalised recommendations