Skip to main content
Log in

The differential effect of normal and pathological aging on egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in navigational and reaching space

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Neurological Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Topographical disorientation (TD) refers to a particular condition which determines the loss of spatial orientation, both in new and familiar environments. TD and spatial memory impairments occur relatively early as effect of cognitive decline in aging, even in prodromal stages of dementia, namely mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Aims

(a) To show that components linked to the recall of familiar spatial knowledge are relatively spared with respect to the learning of unfamiliar ones in normal aging, while they are not in MCI, and (b) to investigate gender differences for their impact on egocentric and allocentric frames of reference.

Method

Forty young participants (YC), 40 healthy elderly participants (HE), 40 elderly participants with subjective memory complaints (SMC), and 40 elderly with probable MCI were administered with egocentric and allocentric familiar tasks, based on the map of their hometown, and with egocentric and allocentric unfamiliar tasks, based on new material to be learned. A series of general linear models were used to analyze data.

Results

No group differences were found on egocentric task based on familiar information. MCI performed worse than the other groups on allocentric tasks based on familiar information (YC = HE = SMC > MCI). Significant differences emerged between groups on egocentric and allocentric tasks based on unfamiliar spatial information (YC > HE = SMC > MCI). A gender difference was found, favoring men on allocentric unfamiliar task.

Conclusion

Familiarity of spatial memory traces can represent a protective factor for retrospective components of TD in normal aging. Conversely, using newly learned information for assessment may lead to overestimating TD severity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Aguirre GK, D’Esposito M (1999) Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain 122(9):1613–1628. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.9.1613

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Pai M, Jacobs WJ (2004) Topographical disorientation in community-residing patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19(3):250–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1081

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L (2014) Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med 275(3):214–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/joim.12190

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Lopez A, Caffò AO, Bosco A (2018) Topographical disorientation in aging. Familiarity with the environment does matter. Neurol Sci 39(9):1519–1528. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-018-3464-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Lopez A, Caffò AO, Spano G, Bosco A (2019) The effect of aging on memory for recent and remote egocentric and allocentric information. Exp Aging Res 45(1):57–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2018.1560117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Boccia M, Nemmi F, Guariglia C (2014) Neuropsychology of environmental navigation in humans: review and meta-analysis of FMRI studies in healthy participants. Neuropsychol Rev 24(2):236–251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9247-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Addis DR, Westmacott R, Grady C, McAndrews MP, Levine B, Black S, Winocur G, Nadel L (2005) Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat 207(1):35–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Evans GW, Pezdek K (1980) Cognitive mapping: knowledge of real-world distance and location information. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn 6(1):13–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.6.1.13

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Thorndyke PW, Hayes-Roth B (1982) Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cogn Psychol 14(4):560–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(82)90019-6

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Merriman NA, Ondřej J, Roudaia E, O’Sullivan C, Newell FN (2016) Familiar environments enhance object and spatial memory in both younger and older adults. Exp Brain Res 234(6):1555–1574. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4557-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Campbell JI, Hepner IJ, Miller LA (2014) The influence of age and sex on memory for a familiar environment. J Environ Psychol 40:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.04.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Kirasic KC (1991) Spatial cognition and behavior in young and elderly adults: implications for learning new environments. Psychol Aging 6(1):10–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.6.1.10

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Rosenbaum RS, Winocur G, Binns MA, Moscovitch M (2012) Remote spatial memory in aging: all is not lost. Front Aging Neurosci 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2012.00025

  14. Meneghetti C, Borella E, Fiore F, De Beni R (2013) The ability to point to well-known places in young and older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res 25(2):203–209. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-013-0027-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Muffato V, Della Giustina M, Meneghetti C, De Beni R (2015) Age-related differences in pointing accuracy in familiar and unfamiliar environments. Cogn Process 16(1):313–317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0720-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Rosenbaum RS, Priselac S, Köhler S, Black SE, Gao F, Nadel L, Moscovitch M (2000) Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nat Neurosci 3(10):1044–1048. https://doi.org/10.1038/79867

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Rosenbaum RS, Gao F, Richards B, Black SE, Moscovitch M (2005) ‘Where to?’ Remote memory for spatial relations and landmark identity in former taxi drivers with Alzheimer’s disease and encephalitis. J Cogn Neurosci 17(3):446–462. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053279496

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. DeIpolyi AR, Rankin KP, Mucke L, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML (2007) Spatial cognition and the human navigation network in AD and MCI. Neurology 69(10):986–997. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000271376.19515.c6

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Hort J, Laczó J, Vyhnálek M, Bojar M, Bureš J, Vlček K (2007) Spatial navigation deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104(10):4042–4047. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0611314104

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Lim TS, Iaria G, Moon SY (2010) Topographical disorientation in mild cognitive impairment: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Clin Neurol 6(4):204–211. https://doi.org/10.3988/jcn.2010.6.4.204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Weniger G, Ruhleder M, Lange C, Wolf S, Irle E (2011) Egocentric and allocentric memory as assessed by virtual reality in individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 49(3):518–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.031

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Benke T, Karner E, Petermichl S, Prantner V, Kemmler G (2014) Neuropsychological deficits associated with route learning in Alzheimer disease, MCI, and normal aging. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 28(2):162–167. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0000000000000009

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Rusconi ML, Suardi A, Zanetti M, Rozzini L (2015) Spatial navigation in elderly healthy subjects, amnestic and non amnestic MCI patients. J Neurol Sci 359(1–2):430–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2015.10.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Boccia M, Silveri MC, Sabatini U, Guariglia C, Nemmi F (2016) Neural underpinnings of the decline of topographical memory in mild cognitive impairment. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement 31(8):618–630. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317516654757

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Caffò AO, Lopez A, Spano G, Serino S, Cipresso P, Stasolla F, Savino M, Lancioni GE, Riva G, Bosco A (2018) Spatial reorientation decline in aging: the combination of geometry and landmarks. Aging Ment Health 22(10):1372–1383. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1354973

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Mohammadi A, Kargar M, Hesami E (2018) Using virtual reality to distinguish subjects with multiple- but not single-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment from normal elderly subjects. Psychogeriatrics 18(2):132–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyg.12301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Reid LM, MacLullich AMJ (2006) Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 22(5–6):471–485. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Youn JC, Kim KW, Lee DY, Jhoo JH, Lee SB, Park JH, Choi EA, Choe JY, Jeong JW, Choo IH, Woo JI (2009) Development of the subjective memory complaints questionnaire. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 27(4):310–317. https://doi.org/10.1159/000205512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Schmand B, Jonker C, Hooijer C, Lindeboom J (1996) Subjective memory complaints may announce dementia. Neurology 46(1):121–125. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.46.1.121

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Portet F, Ousset PJ, Visser PJ, Frisoni GP, Nobili F, Scheltens P, Vellas B, Touchon J, MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer’s Disease (EADC) (2006) Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: a critical review of the concept and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77:714–718. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2005.085332

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Bosco A, Spano G, Caffò AO, Lopez A, Grattagliano I, Saracino G, Pinto K, Hoogeveen F, Lancioni GE (2017) Italians do it worse. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) optimal cut-off scores for people with probable Alzheimer’s disease and with probable cognitive impairment. Aging Clin Exp Res 29(6):1113–1120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40520-017-0727-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Siciliano M, Chiorri C, Passaniti C, Sant’Elia V, Trojano L, Santangelo G (2019) Comparison of alternate and original forms of the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): an Italian normative study. Neurol Sci 40(4):691–702. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-019-3700-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Kawada T (2019) Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) and its memory tasks for detecting mild cognitive impairment. Neurol Sci 40(3):633–633. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-018-3616-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Li X, Jia S, Zhou Z, Jin Y, Zhang X, Hou C, Zheng W, Rong P, Jiao J (2018) The role of the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) and its memory tasks for detecting mild cognitive impairment. Neurol Sci 39(6):1029–1034. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-018-3319-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Katz S (1983) Assessing self-maintenance: activities of daily living, mobility, and instrumental activities of daily living. J Am Geriatr Soc 31(12):721–727. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1983.tb03391.x

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Brink TL, Yesavage JA, Lum O, Heersema PH, Adey M, Rose TL (1982) Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clin Gerontol 1(1):37–43. https://doi.org/10.1300/J018v01n01_06

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lin Y, Shan P, Jiang W, Sheng C, Ma L (2019) Subjective cognitive decline: preclinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurol Sci 40(1):41–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-018-3620-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Schmidt M (1996) Rey auditory verbal learning test: a handbook. Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  39. Appollonio I, Leone M, Isella V, Piamarta F, Consoli T, Villa ML, Forapani E, Russo A, Nichelli P (2005) The frontal assessment battery (FAB): normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci 26(2):108–116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-005-0443-4

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Spinnler H (1987) Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci 6:21–120

    Google Scholar 

  41. Ruggiero G, Iachini T, Ruotolo F, Senese VP (2011) Spatial memory: the role of egocentric and allocentric frames of reference. In J.B. Thomas (Ed.), Spatial memory: visuospatial processes, cognitive performance and developmental effects (Chapter 2, pp. 51–75). New York: Nova Science Publishers

  42. Klencklen G, Després O, Dufour A (2012) What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives. Ageing Res Rev 11(1):123–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2011.10.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Serino S, Cipresso P, Morganti F, Riva G (2014) The role of egocentric and allocentric abilities in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review. Ageing Res Rev 16(1):32–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2014.04.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Sekeres M (2007) Memory consolidation or transformation: context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory. Nat Neurosci 10(5):555–557. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1880

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Hirshhorn M, Newman L, Moscovitch M (2011) Detailed descriptions of routes traveled, but not map-like knowledge, correlates with tests of hippocampal function in older adults. Hippocampus 21(11):1147–1151. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2012.00025

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Colombo D, Serino S, Tuena C, Pedroli E, Dakanalis A, Cipresso P, Riva G (2017) Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 80:605–621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Parizkova M, Lerch O, Moffat SD, Andel R, Mazancova AF, Nedelska Z, Vyhnalek M, Hort J, Laczó J (2018) The effect of Alzheimer’s disease on spatial navigation strategies. Neurobiol Aging 64:107–115

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Lopez A, Caffò AO, Bosco A (2019) Memory for familiar locations: the impact of age, education and cognitive efficiency on two neuropsychological allocentric tasks. Assessment. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119831780

  49. Spreng RN, Lockrow AW, DuPre E, Setton R, Spreng KA, Turner GR (2018) Semanticized autobiographical memory and the default–executive coupling hypothesis of aging. Neuropsychologia 110:37–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Iachini T, Ruotolo F, Vinciguerra M, Ruggiero G (2017) Manipulating time and space: collision prediction in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cognition 166:107–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.024

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Ten Brink AF, Biesbroek JM, Oort Q, Visser-Meily JMA, Nijboer TCW (2019) Peripersonal and extrapersonal visuospatial neglect in different frames of reference: a brain lesion-symptom mapping study. Behav Brain Res 356:504–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.06.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Nori R, Piccardi L, Maialetti A, Goro M, Rossetti A, Argento O, Guariglia C (2018) No gender differences in egocentric and allocentric environmental transformation after compensating for male advantage by manipulating familiarity. Front Neurosci 12:204. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Iachini T, Ruotolo F, Ruggiero G (2009) The effects of familiarity and gender on spatial representation. J Environ Psychol 29(2):227–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.07.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The present study is part of the PhD program of the second author, under the supervision of the last author.

Funding

The first author was supported by the project “Epidemiology of Topographical Disorientation and Mild Cognitive Impairment in a South Italian elderly population”—Action Co-founded by Cohesion and Development Fund 2007–2013—APQ Research Puglia Region “Regional programme supporting smart specialization and social and environmental sustainability—FutureInResearch” (Grant Code CEY4SQ4).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alessandro O. Caffò.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Caffò, A.O., Lopez, A., Spano, G. et al. The differential effect of normal and pathological aging on egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in navigational and reaching space. Neurol Sci 41, 1741–1749 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-020-04261-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-020-04261-4

Keywords

Navigation