Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 6, pp 1171–1183 | Cite as

Mirror self-recognition and its relationship to social cognition in chimpanzees

  • Carla KrachunEmail author
  • Robert Lurz
  • Lindsay M. Mahovetz
  • William D. Hopkins
Original Paper

Abstract

Chimpanzees and humans are capable of recognizing their own reflection in mirrors. Little is understood about the selective pressures that led to this evolved trait and about the mechanisms that underlie it. Here, we investigated the hypothesis that mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees is the byproduct of a developed form of self-awareness that was naturally selected for its adaptive use in social cognitive behaviors. We present here the first direct attempt to assess the social cognition hypothesis by analyzing the association between mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees, as measured by a mirror-mark test, and their performance on a variety of social cognition tests. Consistent with the social cognition hypothesis, chimpanzees who showed evidence of mirror self-recognition in the mark test tended to perform significantly better on the social cognition tasks than those who failed the mark test. Additionally, the data as a whole fit the social cognition hypothesis better than the main competing hypothesis of mirror self-recognition in great apes, the secondary representation hypothesis. Our findings strongly suggest that the evolutionary origins of great apes’ and humans’ capacity to understand ourselves, as revealed by our capacity to recognize ourselves in mirrors, are intimately linked to our ability to understand others.

Keywords

Mirror self-recognition Self-awareness Social cognition Secondary representation Great apes Chimpanzees 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Aaron Kozbelt for his statistical acumen and help with the post hoc power analyses.

Funding

This research was funded by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) Discovery Grant (435593-2013) to C. K., and National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grants (HD-60563 and NS-42867) to W. D. H.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

All four authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable national, international, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. All procedures performed in studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution at which the studies were conducted.

Supplementary material

10071_2019_1309_MOESM1_ESM.docx (1.5 mb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 1527 kb)

References

  1. Allen M, Schwartz BL (2008) Mirror self-recognition in a gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Electron J Integr Biosci 5:19–24Google Scholar
  2. Allman JM, Tetreault NA, Hakeem AY, Manaye KF, Semendeferi K, Erwin JM, Hof PR (2010) The von Economo neurons in the frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex in great apes and humans. Brain Struct Funct 214:495–515PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Allman JM, Tetreault NA, Hakeem A, Park S (2011) The von Economo neurons in apes and humans. Am J Hum Biol 23:5–21PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson JR, Gallup GG Jr (2011) Which primates recognize themselves in mirrors? PLoS Biol 9(3):e1001024PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson JR, Gallup GG Jr (2015) Mirror self-recognition: a review and critique of attempts to promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates 56:317–326PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Ari C, D’Agostini DP (2016) Contingency checking and self-directed behaviors in giant manta rays: do elasmobranchs have self-awareness? J Ethol 34:167Google Scholar
  7. Asendorpf JB, Warkentin V, Baudonniere PM (1996) Self-awareness and other-awareness. II. Mirror self-recognition, social contingency awareness and synchronic imitation. Dev Psychol 32:313–321Google Scholar
  8. Baragli P, Demeru E, Scopa C, Palagi E (2017) Are horses capable of self-recognition? A pilot study. PLoS One 12(5):e0176717PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Bischof-Kohler D (2012) Empathy and self-recognition in phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspective. Emot Rev 4:40–48Google Scholar
  10. Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2007) Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Anim Cogn 10:439–448PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cammaerts M-C, Caemmaerts R (2015) Are ants (Hymenoptera, formicidae) capable of self-recognition? J Sci 5(7):521–532Google Scholar
  12. Chang L, Fang Q, Zhang S, Poo M, Gong N (2015) Mirror-induced self-directed behaviors in rhesus monkeys after visual-somatosensory training. Curr Biol 25:212–217PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chang L, Zhang S, Poo M, Gong N (2017) Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:3258–3263PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Clary D, Kelly D (2016) Graded mirror self-recognition by Clark’s nutcrackers. Sci Rep UK 6:36459Google Scholar
  15. Davidson G, Clayton NS (2015) New perspectives in gaze sensitivity research. Learn Behav 44:9–17Google Scholar
  16. Delfour F, Marten K (2001) Mirror image processing in three marine mammal species: killer whales (Orcinus orca), false killer whales (Pseudorca crassidens) and California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Behav Process 53:181–190Google Scholar
  17. Edler MK, Sherwood CC, Meindl RS, Hopkins WD, Ely JJ, Erwin JM, Raghanti MA (2017) Aged chimpanzees exhibit pathologic hallmarks of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 59:107–120PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Edler MK, Sherwood CC, Meindl RS, Munger EL, Hopkins WD, Ely JJ, Raghanti MA (2018) Microglia changes associated to Alzheimer’s disease pathology in aged chimpanzees. J Comp Neurol 526:2921–2936PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Epstein R, Lanza RP, Skinner BF (1981) “Self-awareness” in the pigeon. Science 212(4495):695–696PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ferrari M, Matthews WS (1983) Self-recognition deficits in autism: syndrome-specific or general developmental delay? J Autism Dev Disord 13:317–324PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Flavell JH (1978) The development of knowledge about visual perception. The Nebraska symposium on motivation, vol 25. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp 43–76Google Scholar
  22. Gallup GG Jr (1970) Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science 167(3914):86–87Google Scholar
  23. Gallup GG Jr (1982) Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. Am J Primatol 2:237–248Google Scholar
  24. Gallup GG Jr (1985) Do minds exist in species other than our own? Neurosci Biobehav R 9:631–641Google Scholar
  25. Gallup GG Jr (1998) Self-awareness and the evolution of social intelligence. Behav Process 42:239–247Google Scholar
  26. Gallup GG Jr, Anderson JR (2018) The “olfactory mirror” and other recent attempts to demonstrate self-recognition in non-primate species. Behav Process 148:16–19Google Scholar
  27. Gallup GG Jr, Povinelli DJ, Suarez SD, Anderson JR, Lethmate J, Menzel EW (1995) Further reflections on self-recognition in primates. Anim Behav 50(6):1525–1532Google Scholar
  28. Gallup GG Jr, Anderson J, Platek SM (2003) Self-awareness, social intelligence and schizophrenia. In: Kircher T, Davis AS (eds) The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 147–165Google Scholar
  29. Gallup GG Jr, Platek S, Spaulding K (2014) The nature of visual self-recognition revisited. Trends Cogn Sci 18:57–58PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition 101:495–514PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Herrmann E, Call J, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M (2007) Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317(5843):1360–1366PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Herrmann E, Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M (2010) Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. PLoS One 5(8):e12438PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Heschl A, Burkart J (2006) A new mark test for mirror self-recognition in non-human primates. Primates 47:187–198PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hopkins WD, Gardner M, Mingle M, Reamer L, Schapiro SJ (2013a) Within- and between-task consistency in hand use as a means of characterizing hand preferences in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 127:380–391PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hopkins WD, Russell JL, McIntyre J, Leavens DL (2013b) Are chimpanzees really so poor at understanding imperative pointing? Some new data and an alternative view of canine and ape social cognition. PLoS One 8(11):379338Google Scholar
  36. Hopkins WD, Keebaugh AC, Reamer LA, Schaeffer J, Schapiro SJ, Young LJ (2014a) Genetic influences on receptive joint attention in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Sci Rep UK 4:3774Google Scholar
  37. Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Schaeffer J (2014b) Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. Curr Biol 24:1–4Google Scholar
  38. Hopkins WD, Reamer L, Mareno MC, Schapiro SJ (2015) Genetic basis for motor skill and hand preference for tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proc R Soc Ser B Biol 782:20141223Google Scholar
  39. Hotta T, Komiyama S, Kohda M (2017) A social cichlid fish failed to pass the mark test. Anim Cogn 21:127–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hyatt CW (1998) Responses of gibbons (Hylobates lar) to their mirror images. Am J Primatol 45:307–311PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Hyatt CW, Hopkins WD (1994) Self-awareness in bonobos and chimpanzees: a comparative perspective. In: Parker ST, Mitchell RW, Boccia ML (eds) Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 248–253Google Scholar
  42. Ikeda Y, Matsumoto G (2007) Mirror image reactions in the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana. Fish Sci 73:1401–1403Google Scholar
  43. Inoue-Nakamura N (1997) Mirror self-recognition in nonhuman primates: a phylogenetic approach. Jpn Psychol Res 39(3):266–275Google Scholar
  44. Keenan J, Gallup GG Jr, Falk D (2003) The face in the mirror: the search for the origins of consciousness. Harper-Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. Lacreuse A, Russell JL, Hopkins WD, Herndon JG (2014) Cognitive and motor aging in female chimpanzees. Neurobiol Aging 35:623–632PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Latzman RD, Sauvigne K, Hopkins WD (2016) Translating chimpanzee personality to humans: investigating the transportability of chimpanzee-derived personality scales to humans. Am J Primatol 78:601–609PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Leavens DA, Reamer LA, Mareno MC, Russell JL, Wilson D, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD (2015) Distal communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence for common ground? Child Dev 86:1623–1638PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Ledbetter DH, Basen JA (1982) Failure to demonstrate self-recognition in gorillas. Am J Primatol 7:307–310Google Scholar
  49. Levensen RW, Miller BL (2007) Loss of cells-loss of self: frontotemporal lobar degeneration and human emotion. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:289–294Google Scholar
  50. Liu X, Liu T, Huang X (2017) Commentary: spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images. Front Psychol 8:1151PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Lurz RW (2009) If chimpanzees are mindreaders, could behavioral science tell? Toward a solution to the logical problem. Philos Psychol 22:305–328Google Scholar
  52. Lurz RW, Krachun C, Mahovetz L, Wilson MJG, Hopkins WD (2018) Chimpanzees gesture to humans in mirrors: using reflection to dissociate seeing from line of gaze. Anim Behav 135:239–249PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Lyn H, Russell JL, Leavens DA, Bard KA, Boysen ST, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2014) Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: methodology matters. Anim Cogn 17:85–94PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Ma X, Jin Y, Luo B, Zhang G, Wei R, Liu D (2015) Giant pandas failed to show mirror self-recognition. Anim Cogn 18:713–721PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Mahovetz LM, Young LY, Hopkins WD (2016) The influence of AVPR1A genotype on individual differences in behaviors during a mirror self-recognition tasks in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Genes Brain Behav 15:445–452PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Mitchell RW (1993) Mental models of mirror-self recognition: two theories. New Ideas Psychol 11:295–325Google Scholar
  57. Mitchell RW (1997) A comparison of the self-awareness and kinesthetic matching theories of self-recognition: autistic children and others. Ann N Y Acad Sci 818:39–62Google Scholar
  58. Nichols KE, Fox NA, Mundy P (2005) Joint attention, self-recognition and neurocognitive function in toddlers. Infancy 7:35–51Google Scholar
  59. Nielsen M, Collier-Baker E, Davis JM, Suddendorf T (2005) Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 8:31–36PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Nimchinsky EA, Gilissen E, Allman JM, Perl DP, Erwin JM, Hof PR (1999) A neuronal morphology type unique to humans and great apes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:5268–5273PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Parker S (1996) Apprenticeship in tool-mediated extractive foraging: the origins of imitation, teaching, and self-awareness in great apes. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 348–370Google Scholar
  62. Parker S, Mitchell RW, Boccia MC (eds) (1994) Self-awareness in animals and humans. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  63. Patterson FGP, Cohen RH (1994) Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas. In: Parker ST, Mitchell RW, Boccia ML (eds) Self-awareness in animals and humans. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 273–290Google Scholar
  64. Perner J (2012) MiniMeta: In search of minimal criteria for metacognition. In: Beran MJ, Brandl J, Perner J, Proust J (eds) Foundations of metacognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 94–116Google Scholar
  65. Plotnik JM, de Waal FB, Reiss D (2006) Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:17053–17057PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Pope SM, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2015) The association between imitation recognition and socio-communicative competencies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Front Psychol 6:188PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  67. Posada S, Colell M (2007) Another gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) recognizes himself in the mirror. Am J Primatol 69:576–583PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Povinelli J, Cant J (1995) Arboreal clambering and the evolution of self-conception. Q Rev Biol 70:393–421PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Povinelli DJ, Rulf AB, Landau KR, Bierschwale DT (1993) Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. J Comp Psychol 107:347–372PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Prior H, Schwarz A, Güntürkün O (2008) Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biol 6(8):e202PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  71. Rajala AZ, Reininger KR, Lancaster KM, Populin LC (2010) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) do recognize themselves in the mirror: implications for the evolution of self-recognition. PLoS One 5(9):e12865PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  72. Reiss D, Marino L (2001) Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5937–5942PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Repacholi BM, Gopnik A (1997) Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- to 18-month-olds. Dev Psychol 33:12–21PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Russell JL, Lyn H, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2011) The role of socio-communicative rearing environments in the development of social and physical cognition in apes. Dev Sci 14:1459–1470PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  75. Saxe R, Moran JM, Scholz J, Gabrieli J (2006) Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. Soc Cogn Affect Neur 1:229–234Google Scholar
  76. Schmitt V, Pankau B, Fischer J (2011) Old world monkeys compare to apes in the primate cognition test battery. PLoS One 7(4):e32024Google Scholar
  77. Seeley WW, Carlin DA, Allman JM, Macedo MN, Bush C, Miller BL, Dearmond SJ (2006) Early frontotemporal dementia targets neurons unique to apes and humans. Ann Neurol 60:660–667PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Soler M, Pérez-Contreras T, Peralta-Sánchez JM (2014) Mirror-mark tests performed on jackdaws reveal potential methodological problems in the use of stickers in avian mark-test studies. PLoS One 9(1):e86193PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  79. Spiker D, Ricks M (1984) Visual self-recognition in autistic children: developmental relationships. Child Dev 55:214–225PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Suddendorf T (1998) Simpler for evolution: secondary representation in apes, children, and ancestors. Behav Brain Sci 21:131Google Scholar
  81. Suddendorf T, Butler DL (2013) The nature of visual self-recognition. Trends Cogn Sci 17:121–127PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Suddendorf T, Collier-Baker E (2009) The evolution of primate visual self-recognition: evidence of absence in lesser apes. Proc R Soc Ser B Biol 276:1671–1677Google Scholar
  83. Suddendorf T, Whiten A (2001) Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychol Bull 127:629–650PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Swartz KB, Evans S (1991) Not all chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) show self-recognition. Primates 32:483–496Google Scholar
  85. Tomasello M, Hare B, Agnetta B (1999) Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Anim Behav 58:769–777PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. van Buuren M, Auersperg A, Gajdon G, Tebbich S, von Bayern A (2018) No evidence of mirror self-recognition in keas and Goffin’s cockatoos. Behaviour.  https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003514 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. van Veluw SJ, Chance SA (2014) Differentiating between self and others: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging Behav 8:24–38PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Wellman HM (2014) Making minds: how theory of mind develops. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  89. Wellman HM, Phillips AT, Rodriguez T (2000) Young children’s understanding of perception, desire and emotion. Child Dev 71:895–912PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Whiten A (2013) Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world. Anim Behav 86:213–221Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of SaskatchewanSaskatoonCanada
  2. 2.Department of Philosophy, Brooklyn CollegeCUNYBrooklynUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of North FloridaJacksonvilleUSA
  4. 4.Department of Comparative MedicineUniversity of Texas MD Anderson Cancer CenterBastropUSA

Personalised recommendations