Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 6, pp 947–958 | Cite as

Memory for own behaviour in pinnipeds

  • Simeon Quirinus SmeeleEmail author
  • Kirstin Anderson Hansen
  • Sara Torres Ortiz
  • Fredrik Johansson
  • Jakob Højer Kristensen
  • Josefin Larsson
  • Ursula Siebert
  • Magnus Wahlberg
Original Paper

Abstract

Pinnipeds are aquatic predators feeding on a vast range of prey, and their social behaviour differs greatly between species (from extreme polygyny in some sea lions to monogamy in some true seals). It has been hypothesised that the foraging and social complexity of their lifestyle should drive the evolution of their cognitive abilities. To investigate how aware pinnipeds are of their own behaviour, a grey seal (Halichoerus grypus), two harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) and four South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens) were trained to repeat their own behaviour on command. Three already trained behaviours were used, and the animal was asked to repeat the behaviour twice to ensure that the animal recalled its own behaviour and not the command given for the previous behaviour. All three species could recall their own behaviour significantly better than by chance. The duration for which the animals could recall their behaviour was tested using a staircase paradigm. A delay was implemented between the completion of the behaviour and the command to repeat it. The delay was increased after correct responses and decreased after incorrect responses. The performance of all species fell towards chance level after 12–18 s, with no significant difference between species. These results indicate that sea lions and true seals are aware of their own behaviour and that true seals have similar short-term memory abilities. It also shows that pinnipeds have less developed short-term memory abilities compared to other aquatic predators, such as the bottlenose dolphin. The complexity of pinniped foraging and social behaviour does not seem to have driven the evolution of short-term memory abilities in these animals but might have contributed to their ability to recall their own behaviour.

Keywords

Cognition Seal Sea lion Self-awareness Short-term memory 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Wayne Phillips, Benjamin Doshner and Omar del Oso from Dolphin adventure for their support and for the predisposition of the animals in Dolphin Adventure. We also thank Abel Reyes, Jesus Manuel Perez, Jesus Silva, Kelli Rust, Luis Hernández and Paola Valle for training the animals and providing their session time with them.

Funding

This study was funded by the Danish Research Council (Grant No. 400200536) and the Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research (ITAW) University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Foundation (Grant No. 60079084).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. The animals from Dolphin Adventure were kept under permits from Mexican Secretariat of Environment and Natural Resources [permit: INE/CITES/DGVS-EF-P-0033-NAY/00 (PIMVS)]. The animals from Fjord&Bælt (permit: J.nr.SVANA-610-00084) and The University of Southern Denmark (permit: 2300-50120-00003-09) were kept under permits from the Danish Ministry of Food and Agriculture. The experiments did not require an application to the Animal Ethics Committee of neither Denmark nor Mexico, as animals participated voluntarily in the experiments and were not affected by them in any way. This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Asselin S, Hammill MO, Barrette C (1993) Underwater vocalizations of ice breeding gray seals. Can J Zool 71(11):2211–2219Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley A, Eysenck MW, Anderson MC (2010) Memory. East Sussex, Psychology Press, HoveGoogle Scholar
  3. Beninger RJ, Kendall SB, Vanderwolf CH (1974) The ability of rats to discriminate their own behaviours. Can J Psychol/Rev Can Psychol 28(1):79–91Google Scholar
  4. Berg I, Haug T, Nilssen KT (2002) Harbour Seal (Phoca vitulina) Diet in Vester » len, North Norway. Sarsia N Atl Mar Sci 87(6):451–461Google Scholar
  5. Bishop AM, Onoufriou J, Moss S, Pomeroy PP, Twiss SD (2016) Cannibalism by a Male Grey Seal (Halichoerus grypus) in the North Sea. Aquat Mamm 42(2):137–143Google Scholar
  6. Boddy A, McGowen M, Sherwood C, Grossman L, Goodman M, Wildman D (2012) Comparative analysis of encephalization in mammals reveals relaxed constraints on anthropoid primate and cetacean brain scaling. J Evol Biol 25(5):981–994PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowen WD, Beck CA, Austin DA (2009) Pinniped ecology. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 533–541Google Scholar
  8. Brownlow A, Onoufriou J, Bishop A, Davison N, Thompson D (2016) Corkscrew seals: grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) infanticide and cannibalism may indicate the cause of spiral lacerations in seals. Plos One 11(6):e0156464PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Burns JJ (2009) Harbor Seal and spotted seal Phoca vitulina and P. largha. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 533–541Google Scholar
  10. Cappozzo HL, Perrin WF (2009) South American Sea Lion: Otaria flavescens. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals, 2nd edn. Academic Press, London, pp 1076–1079Google Scholar
  11. Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1980) Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool 190(Mar):309–323Google Scholar
  12. Cowan RB (2003) Short term memory for behavior in the Bottlenose Dolphin (Turisops truncates). Psychology. University of Hawaii at Manoa, Manoa, p 80Google Scholar
  13. Dunbar RI, Shultz S (2007) Evolution in the social brain. Science 317(5843):1344–1347PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Eisenberg JF, Wilson DE (1978) Relative brain size and feeding strategies in the Chiroptera. Evolution 32(4):740–751PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fox KC, Muthukrishna M, Shultz S (2017) The social and cultural roots of whale and dolphin brains. Nat Ecol Evol 1(11):1699PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Haelters J, Kerckhof F, Jauniaux T, Degraer S (2012) The Grey Seal (Halichoerus grypus) as a Predator of Harbour Porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)? Aquat Mamm 38(4):343–353Google Scholar
  17. Hall A, Thompson D (2009) Grey seal Halichoerus grypus. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 501–503Google Scholar
  18. Hall AJ, Watkins J, Hammond PS (1998) Seasonal variation in the diet of harbour seals in the south-western North Sea. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 170:269–281Google Scholar
  19. Heers T, van Neer A, Becker A, Gross S, Hansen KA, Siebert U, Abdulmawjood A (2018) Loop-mediated isothermal amplification (LAMP) as a confirmatory and rapid DNA detection method for grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) predation on harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). J Sea Res 140:32–39Google Scholar
  20. Higdon JW, Bininda-Emonds OR, Beck RM, Ferguson SH (2007) Phylogeny and divergence of the pinnipeds (Carnivora: Mammalia) assessed using a multigene dataset. BMC Evol Biol 7(1):216PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Jouventin P, Cornet A (1980) The sociobiology of pinnipeds. Adv Study Behav 11:121–141Google Scholar
  22. Kazdin AE (2012) Behavior modification in applied settings. Waveland Press, Long GroveGoogle Scholar
  23. Konigson S, Fjalling A, Berglind M, Lunneryd SG (2013) Male gray seals specialize in raiding salmon traps. Fish Res 148:117–123Google Scholar
  24. Lehtonen E, Suuronen P (2010) Live-capture of grey seals in a modified salmon trap. Fish Res 102(1–2):214–216Google Scholar
  25. Leopold MF, Begeman L, van Bleijswijk JDL, Ijsseldijk LL, Witte HJ, Grone A (2015) Exposing the grey seal as a major predator of harbour porpoises. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 282(1798):20142429Google Scholar
  26. Lundström K, Hjerne O, Alexandersson K, Karlsson O (2007) Estimation of grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) diet composition in the Baltic Sea. NAMMCO Sci Publ 6:177–196Google Scholar
  27. Marino L (1998) A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans and anthropoid primates. Brain Behav Evol 51(4):230–238PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Marino L (2002) Convergence of complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and primates. Brain Behav Evol 59(1–2):21–32PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Martín HF (2016) Self-cognition in marine mammals studied by the mirror test. Master thesis, Department of Biology, Odense University of Southern DenmarkGoogle Scholar
  30. Mauck B, Dehnhardt G (2007) Spatial multiple-choice matching in a harbour seal (Phoca vitulina): differential encoding of landscape versus local feature information? Anim Cogn 10(4):397–405PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Mercado E, Murray SO, Uyeyama RK, Pack AA, Herman LM (1998) Memory for recent actions in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): repetition of arbitrary behaviors using an abstract rule. Anim Learn Behav 26(2):210–218Google Scholar
  32. Mercado E, Uyeyama RK, Pack AA, Herman LM (1999) Memory for action events in the bottlenosed dolphin. Anim Cogn 2:17–25Google Scholar
  33. Mikkelsen B, Haug T, Nilssen KT (2002) Summer diet of grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) in Faroese waters. Sarsia N Atlant Mar Sci 87(6):462–471Google Scholar
  34. Morin A (2006) Levels of consciousness and self-awareness: a comparison and integration of various neurocognitive views. Conscious Cogn 15(2):358–371PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Pack AA, Herman LM, Roitblat HL (1991) Generalization of visual matching and delayed matching by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Learn Behav 19(1):37–48Google Scholar
  36. Paukner A, Anderson JR, Donaldson DI, Ferrari PF (2007) Cued repetition of self-directed behaviors in macaques (Macaca nemestrina). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 33(2):139PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Pryor K, Callenbach EBE, Davis GW (2002) Don’t shoot the dog: how to improve yourself and others through behavioral training, revised edition. Ringpress Books Ltd., GloucestershireGoogle Scholar
  38. Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S (1985) Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, phoca-vitulina. Can J Zool 63(5):1050–1056Google Scholar
  39. Sepúlveda M, Pérez M, Sielfeld W, Oliva D, Durán L, Rodríguez L, Araos V, Buscaglia M (2007) Operational interaction between South American sea lions Otaria flavescens and artisanal (small-scale) fishing in Chile: results from interview surveys and on-board observations. Fish Res 83(2–3):332–340Google Scholar
  40. Shettleworth SJ (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  41. Shimp CP (1982) On metaknowledge in the pigeon: an organism’s knowledge about its own behavior. Anim Learn Behav 10(3):358–364Google Scholar
  42. Stansbury A (2015) Vocal learning and development in the grey seal, Halichoerus grypus. University of St Andrews, St AndrewsGoogle Scholar
  43. van Neer A, Jensen LF, Siebert U (2015) Grey seal (Halichoerus grypus) predation on harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) on the island of Helgoland, Germany. J Sea Res 97:1–4Google Scholar
  44. Zentall TR, Smith AP (2016) Delayed matching-to-sample: a tool to assess memory and other cognitive processes in pigeons. Behav Proc 123:26–42Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Simeon Quirinus Smeele
    • 1
    Email author
  • Kirstin Anderson Hansen
    • 1
    • 2
  • Sara Torres Ortiz
    • 1
    • 3
  • Fredrik Johansson
    • 4
  • Jakob Højer Kristensen
    • 4
  • Josefin Larsson
    • 4
  • Ursula Siebert
    • 2
  • Magnus Wahlberg
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of Southern DenmarkOdenseDenmark
  2. 2.Institute for Terrestrial and Aquatic Wildlife Research (ITAW)University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, FoundationBüsumGermany
  3. 3.Dolphin Adventure, Vallarta AdventuresNuevo VallartaMexico
  4. 4.Fjord&BæltKertemindeDenmark

Personalised recommendations