Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 21, Issue 6, pp 773–785 | Cite as

Variability in the “stereotyped” prey capture sequence of male cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) could relate to personality differences

  • Francesca ZorattoEmail author
  • Giulia Cordeschi
  • Giacomo Grignani
  • Roberto Bonanni
  • Enrico Alleva
  • Giuseppe Nascetti
  • Jennifer A. Mather
  • Claudio Carere
Original Paper

Abstract

Studies of animal personality have shown consistent between-individual variation in behaviour in many social and non-social contexts, but hunting behaviour has been overlooked. Prey capture sequences, especially in invertebrates, are supposed to be quite invariant. In cuttlefish, the attack includes three components: attention, positioning, and seizure. The previous studies indicated some variability in these components and we quantified it under the hypothesis that it could relate to personality differences. We, therefore, analysed predation sequences of adult cuttlefish to test their association with personality traits in different contexts. Nineteen subjects were first exposed to an “alert” and a “threat” test and then given a live prey, for 10 days. Predation sequences were scored for components of the attack, locomotor and postural elements, body patterns, and number of successful tentacle ejections (i.e. seizure). PCA analysis of predatory patterns identified three dimensions accounting for 53.1%, 15.9%, and 9.6% of the variance and discriminating individuals based on “speed in catching prey”, “duration of attack behaviour”, and “attention to prey”. Predation rate, success rate, and hunting time were significantly correlated with the first, second, and third PCA factors, respectively. Significant correlations between capture patterns and responsiveness in the alert and threat tests were found, highlighting a consistency of prey capture patterns with measures of personality in other contexts. Personality may permeate even those behaviour patterns that appear relatively invariant.

Keywords

Cephalopods Hunting behaviour Animal personality Behavioural consistency Welfare 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was partially supported by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Individual Fellowships (H2020-MSCA-IF-2014), Project ID: 659106 (GROUPIND), to C. Carere. We wish to thank A. Giuliani and F. Chiarotti for statistical advice, N. Francia and S. Falsini for precious technical and administrative support. The work is part of the BSc thesis of G. Cordeschi. Finally, we wish to thank the authors of the original study (Carere et al. 2015).

Funding

This study was funded by H2020-MSCA-IF-2014 (GROUPIND), Grant number 659106.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals, applicable when the original study by Carere et al. (2015) was performed, were followed. Specifically, the original study complied with the regulations of the Canadian Council of Animal Care for animal research (University of Lethbridge, Animal Welfare Approval n. 1106).

Supplementary material

10071_2018_1209_MOESM1_ESM.pptx (1 mb)
Supplementary material 1 (PPTX 1045 KB)
10071_2018_1209_MOESM2_ESM.docx (35 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (DOCX 35 KB)

References

  1. Adamo SA, Ehgoetz K, Sangster C, Whitehorne I (2006) Signaling to the enemy? Body pattern expression and its response to external cue during hunting in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda). Biol Bull 210:192–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson RC, Mather JA, Steele CW (2004) Burying and associated behaviors of Rossia pacifica (Cephalopoda: Sepiolidae). Vie et Milieu Serie A Biologie Marine 54:13–19Google Scholar
  3. Araujo MS, Bolnick DI, Martinelli LA, Giaretta AA, dos Reis SF (2009) Individual-level diet variation in four species of Brazilian frogs. J Anim Ecol 78:848–856CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barlow GW (1968) Ethological units of behaviour. In: Ingle D (ed) The central nervous system and fish behaviour. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 217–237Google Scholar
  5. Barlow GW (1977) Modal action patterns. In: Sebeok TA (ed) How animals communicate. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp 98–134Google Scholar
  6. Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL (2009) The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 77:771–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berridge KC (1990) Comparative fine-structure of action-rules of form and sequence in the grooming patterns of 6 rodent species. Behaviour 113:21–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boal JG, Wittenberg KM, Hanlon RT (2000) Observational learning does not explain improvement in predation tactics by cuttlefish (Mollusca: Cephalopoda). Behav Proc 52:141–153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bonner JT (1980) The evolution of culture in animals. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  10. Boycott BB (1953) The chromatophore system of cephalopods. Proc Linn Soc Lond 164:235–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boycott BB (1958) The cuttlefish—Sepia. New Biol 25:98–118Google Scholar
  12. Brown C, Garwood MP, Williamson JE (2012) It pays to cheat: tactical deception in a cephalopod social signaling system. Biol Lett 8:729–732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carere C, Eens M (2005) Unravelling animal personalities: how and why individuals consistently differ. Behaviour 142:1149–1157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carere C, Maestripieri D (eds) (2013) Animal personalities: behavior physiology and evolution. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  15. Carere C, Grignani G, Bonanni R, Della Gala M, Carlini A, Angeletti D, Cimmaruta R, Nascetti G, Mather JA (2015) Consistent individual differences in the behavioural responsiveness of adult male cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Appl Anim Behav Sci 167:89–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chittka L, Skorupski P, Raine NE (2009) Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in animal decision making. Trends Ecol Evol 24(7):400–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Coleman K, Wilson DS (1998) Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific. Anim Behav 56:927–936CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Costantini D, Casagrande S, Di Lieto G, Fanfani A, Dell’Omo G (2005) Consistent differences in feeding habits between neighbouring breeding kestrels. Behaviour 142:1403–1415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Crossland MR (2001) Ability of predatory native Australian fishes to learn to avoid toxic larvae of the introduced toad Bufo marinus. J Fish Biol 59:319–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Darmaillacq AS, Dickel L, Chichery MP, Chichery R (2004) Rapid taste aversion learning in adult cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Anim Behav 68:1291–1298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Darmaillacq AS, Jozet-Alves C, Bellanger C, Dickel L (2014) Cuttlefish preschool or how to learn in the peri-hatching period. In: Darmaillacq AS, Dickel L, Mather JM (eds) Cephalopod cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 3–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dickel L, Chichery MP, Chichery R (1997) Postembryonic maturation of the vertical lobe complex and early development of predatory behavior in the cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Neurobiol Learn Mem 67:150–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dixon LM, Duncan IJH, Mason G (2008) What’s in a peck? Using fixed action pattern morphology to identify the motivational basis of abnormal feather-pecking behaviour. Anim Behav 76:1035–1042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Duval P, Chichery MP, Chichery R (1984) Prey capture by the cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L): an experimental study of two strategies. Behav Proc 9:13–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ferrari C, Pasquaretta C, Carere C, Cavallone E, von Hardenberg A, Reale D (2013) Testing for the presence of coping styles in a wild mammal. Anim Behav 85:1385–1396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fiorito G, Affuso A, Anderson DB, Basil J, Bonnaud L, Botta G, Cole A et al (2014) Cephalopods in neuroscience: regulations research and the 3Rs. Invertebr Neurosci 14:13–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fiorito G, Affuso A, Basil J, Cole A, de Girolamo P, D’Angelo L, Dickel L et al (2015) Guidelines for the care and welfare of cephalopods in research—a consensus based on an initiative by CephRes FELASA and the Boyd Group. Lab Anim 49:1–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Flash T, Hochner B (2005) Motor primitives in vertebrates and invertebrates. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:660–666CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Giuliani A (2017) The application of principal component analysis to drug discovery and biomedical data. Drug Discov Today 22:1069–1076CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Giuliani A, Ghirardi O, Caprioli A, Di Serio S, Ramacci MT, Angelucci L (1994) Multivariate analysis of behavioural aging highlights some unexpected features of complex systems organization. Behav Neural Biol 61:110–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Giuliani A, Zbilut JP, Conti F, Manetti C, Miccheli A (2004) Invariant features of metabolic networks: a data analysis application on scaling properties of biochemical pathways. Phys A 337:157–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gosling SD (2001) From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychol Bull 127:45–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Griffin A, Healy SD, Guillette LM (2015) Cognition and personality: an analysis of an emerging field. Trends in Ecology Evolution 30:207–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Groothuis TGG, Carere C (2005) Avian personalities: characterisation and epigenesist. Neurosci Biobehav Re 29:137–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Guerra A (2006) Ecology of Sepia officinalis. Vie et Milieu Serie A Biologie Marine 56:97–107Google Scholar
  36. Guillette LM, Naguib M, Griffin AS (2017) Individual differences in behaviour and cognition. Behav Proc 134:1–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hanlon RT, Messenger JB (1988) Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.): the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B 320:437–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hanlon RT, Messenger JB (1996) Cephalopod behaviour. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  39. Holmes W (1940) The colour and the colour patterns of Sepia officinalis. Proc Zool Soc Lond 110:17–36Google Scholar
  40. Horvath K, Angeletti D, Nascetti G, Carere C (2013) Invertebrate welfare: an overlooked issue. Annali dell’Istituto Superiore di Sanità 49:9–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Huntingford FA (1984) Some ethical issues raised by studies of predation and aggression. Anim Behav 32:210–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lessells CM, Boag PT (1987) Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk 104:116–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lorenz K, Tinbergen N (1938) Taxis and instinctive behaviour pattern in egg-rolling by the Greylag goose. In: Lorenz K (ed) Studies in animal and human behaviour, vol 1. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 316–350Google Scholar
  44. Mather JA (1986) Sand-digging in Sepia officinalis: assessment of a cephalopod mollusc’s “fixed” behavior pattern. J Comp Psychol 100:315–320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mather JA (2011) Consciousness in cephalopods? J Cosmol 14:4472–4483Google Scholar
  46. Mather JA, Anderson RC (1993) Personalities of Octopuses (Octopus rubescens). J Comp Psychol 107:336–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mather JA, Carere C (2018) Consider the individual: personality and welfare in invertebrate animals. In: Carere C, Mather JA (eds) The welfare of invertebrate animals. Springer International Publishing, Cham (in press) Google Scholar
  48. Mather JA, Logue DM (2013) The bold and the spineless: approaches to personality in invertebrates. In: Carere C, Maestripieri D (eds) Animal personalities: behavior physiology and evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (IL), pp 13–35Google Scholar
  49. Mather JA, Leite TS, Batista AT (2012) Individual prey choices of octopuses: are they generalist or specialist? Curr Zool 58:597–603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Messenger JB (1968) The visual attack of the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Anim Behav 16:342–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Nakagawa S (2004) A farewell to Bonferroni: the problems of low statistical power and publication bias. Behav Ecol 15:1044–1045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Natoli E, Say L, Cafazzo S, Bonanni R, Schmid M, Pointier D (2005) Bold attitude makes urban feral domestic cats more vulnerable to feline immunodeficiency virus. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:151–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. O’Brien CE, Mezrai N, Darmaillacq AS, Dickel L (2016) Behavioral development in embryonic and early juvenile cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Dev Psychobiol 9999:1–16Google Scholar
  54. Pronk R, Wilson DR, Harcourt R (2010) Video playback demonstrates episodic personality in the gloomy octopus. J Exp Biol 213:1035–1041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Reale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall OT, Dingemanse NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev 82:291–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Sanders FK, Young JZ (1940) Learning and other functions of the higher nervous centres of Sepia. J Neurophysiol 3:501–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sih A, Del Giudice M (2012) Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: a behavioural ecology perspective. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:2762–2772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sih A, Bell A, Chadwick JC (2004) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol Evol 19:372–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sinn DL, Moltschaniwskyji NA (2005) Personality traits in dumpling squid (Euprymna tasmanica): context-specific traits and their correlation with biological characteristics. J Comp Psychol 119:99–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sinn D, Perrin N, Mather JA, Anderson RC (2001) Early temperamental traits in an octopus (Octopus bimaculoides). J Comp Psychol 115:351–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Smith JA, Andrews PLR, Hawkins P, Louhimies S, Ponte G, Dickel L (2013) Cephalopod research and EU Directive 2010/63/EU: requirements impacts and ethical review processes. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 447:31–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Tetley CL, O’Hara SJ (2012) Rating of personality as a tool for improving the breeding management and welfare of zoo animals. Anim Welf 21:463–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tonkins BM, Tyers AM, Cooke GM (2015) Cuttlefish in captivity: an investigation into housing and husbandry for improving welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci 168:77–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. van Hierden YM, Korte SM, Ruesink EW, van Reenen CG, Engel B, Koolhaas JM, Blokhuis HJ (2002) The development of feather pecking behaviour and targeting of pecking in chicks from a high and low feather pecking line of laying hens. Appl Anim Behav Sci 77:183–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. van Oers K, de Jong G, Drent PJ, van Noordwijk AJ (2004) Genetic correlations of avian personality traits: correlated response to artificial selection. Behav Genet 34:611–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. von Boletzky S, Hanlon RT (1983) A review of the laboratory maintenance rearing and culture of cephalopod molluscs. Mem Natl Mus Vic 44:147–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wells MJ (1958) Factors affecting reactions to mysis by newly hatched Sepia. Behaviour 13:96–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wilson DP (1946) A note on the capture of prey by Sepia officinalis L. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 26:421–425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Yang T-I, Chiao C-C (2016) Number sense and state-dependent valuation in cuttlefish. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 283:20161379CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Behavioural Sciences and Mental HealthIstituto Superiore di SanitàRomeItaly
  2. 2.Ichthyogenic Experimental Marine Centre (CISMAR), Department of Ecological and Biological SciencesUniversity of TusciaTarquiniaItaly
  3. 3.Independent ResearcherRomeItaly
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyUniversity of LethbridgeLethbridgeCanada
  5. 5.Laboratory of Experimental and Comparative EthologyUniversity of Paris 13, Sorbonne Paris CitéVilletaneuseFrance

Personalised recommendations