Skip to main content
Log in

Food approach conditioning and discrimination learning using sound cues in benthic sharks

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The marine environment is filled with biotic and abiotic sounds. Some of these sounds predict important events that influence fitness while others are unimportant. Individuals can learn specific sound cues and ‘soundscapes’ and use them for vital activities such as foraging, predator avoidance, communication and orientation. Most research with sounds in elasmobranchs has focused on hearing thresholds and attractiveness to sound sources, but very little is known about their abilities to learn about sounds, especially in benthic species. Here we investigated if juvenile Port Jackson sharks could learn to associate a musical stimulus with a food reward, discriminate between two distinct musical stimuli, and whether individual personality traits were linked to cognitive performance. Five out of eight sharks were successfully conditioned to associate a jazz song with a food reward delivered in a specific corner of the tank. We observed repeatable individual differences in activity and boldness in all eight sharks, but these personality traits were not linked to the learning performance assays we examined. These sharks were later trained in a discrimination task, where they had to distinguish between the same jazz and a novel classical music song, and swim to opposite corners of the tank according to the stimulus played. The sharks’ performance to the jazz stimulus declined to chance levels in the discrimination task. Interestingly, some sharks developed a strong side bias to the right, which in some cases was not the correct side for the jazz stimulus.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Banner A (1972) Use of sound in predation by young lemon sharks, Negaprion brevirostris (Poey). Bull Mar Sci 22:251–283

    Google Scholar 

  • Bass NC, Mourier J, Knott NA, Day J, Guttridge T, Brown C (2016) Long-term migration patterns and bisexual philopatry in a benthic shark species. Mar Freshw Res 68:1414–1421

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint. arXiv:1406.5823

  • Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, White J-SS (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 24:127–135

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vis 10:433–436

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown C, Laland K, Krause J (2011) Fish cognition and behavior. Wiley, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Budaev S, Brown C (2011) Personality traits and behaviour. In: Brown C, Laland K, Krause J (eds) Fish cognition and behavior. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, p 472

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes EE, Brown C (2016) Individual personality differences in Port Jackson sharks Heterodontus portusjacksoni. J Fish Biol 89:1142–1157

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes EE, Vila Pouca C, Chambers SL, Brown C (2016) Into the wild: developing field tests to examine the link between elasmobranch personality and laterality. Behav 153:1777–1793

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carere C, Locurto C (2011) Interaction between animal personality and animal cognition. Curr Zool 57:491–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casper BM, Mann DA (2006) Evoked potential audiograms of the nurse shark (Ginglymostoma cirratum) and the yellow stingray (Urobatis jamaicensis). Environ Biol Fishes 76:101–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casper BM, Mann DA (2007) Dipole hearing measurements in elasmobranch fishes. J Exp Biol 210:75–81

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casper B, Mann D (2009) Field hearing measurements of the Atlantic sharpnose shark Rhizoprionodon terraenovae. J Fish Biol 75:2768–2776

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chase AR (2001) Music discriminations by carp (Cyprinus carpio). Anim Learn Behav 29:336–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford JD, Hagedorn M, Hopkins CD (1986) Acoustic communication in an electric fish, Pollimyrus isidori (Mormyridae). J Comp Physiol A 159:297–310

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato MR, Salmon DP (1984) Processing of complex auditory stimuli (tunes) by rats and monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Learn Behav 12:184–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA, Alfieri MS (2003) Boldness, behavioral inhibition and learning. Ethol Ecol Evol 15:43–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster CB, Skinner BF (1957) Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts, East Norwalk, CT, USA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Finger JS, Dhellemmes F, Guttridge TL (2017) Personality in elasmobranchs with a focus on sharks: Early evidence, challenges, and future directions. In: Vonk J, Weiss A, Kuczaj SA (eds) Personality in nonhuman animals. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 129–152

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gannon DP, Barros NB, Nowacek DP, Read AJ, Waples DM, Wells RS (2005) Prey detection by bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: an experimental test of the passive listening hypothesis. Anim Behav 69:709–720

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner JM, Hueter RE, Maruska KP, Sisneros JA, Casper BM, Mann DA, Demski LS (2012) Sensory physiology and behavior of elasmobranchs. Biol Sharks Relat 1:349–401

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttridge TL, Brown C (2014) Learning and memory in the Port Jackson shark, Heterodontus portusjacksoni. Anim Cogn 17:415–425

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart NS, Collin SP (2015) Sharks senses and shark repellents. Integr Zool 10:38–64

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart NS, Theiss SM, Harahush BK, Collin SP (2011) Microspectrophotometric evidence for cone monochromacy in sharks. Naturwissenschaften 98:193–201

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huijbers CM, Nagelkerken I, Lössbroek PAC, Schulten IE, Siegenthaler A, Holderied MW, Simpson SD (2012) A test of the senses: fish select novel habitats by responding to multiple cues. Ecology 93:46–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hulse SH, Takeuchi AH, Braaten RF (1992) Perceptual invariances in the comparative psychology of music. Music Percept 10:151–184. https://doi.org/10.2307/40285605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan LK, Mandelman JW, Kajiura SM (2011) Behavioral responses to weak electric fields and a lanthanide metal in two shark species. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 409:345–350

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly JC, Nelson DR (1975) Hearing thresholds of the horn shark, Heterodontus francisci. J Acoust Soc Am 58:905–909

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kritzler H, Wood L (1961) Provisional audiogram for the shark, Carcharhinus leucas. Science 133:1480–1482

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ladich F (2015) Sound communication in fishes. Animal signals and communication series, vol. 4. Springer, Vienna

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ladich F, Myrberg A (2006) Agonistic behavior and acoustic communication. Commun Fish 1:121–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Last PR, Stevens JD (2009) Sharks and rays of Australia. CSIRO Publishing, Australia, 644p

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefcheck JS (2016) piecewiseSEM: piecewise structural equation modelling in r for ecology, evolution, and systematics. Methods Ecol Evol 7:573–579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann DA, Lu Z, Popper AN (1997) A clupeid fish can detect ultrasound. Nature 389:341–341

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McFarland W (1990) Light in the sea: the optical world of elasmobranchs. J Exp Zool 256:3–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer CG, Holland KN, Papastamatiou YP (2005) Sharks can detect changes in the geomagnetic field. J R Soc Interface 2:129–130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myrberg AA Jr, Ha SJ, Walewski S, Banbury JC (1972) Effectiveness of acoustic signals in attracting epipelagic sharks to an underwater sound source. Bull Mar Sci 22:926–949

    Google Scholar 

  • Myrberg AA Jr, Mohler M, Catala JD (1986) Sound production by males of a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus): its significance to females. Anim Behav 34:913–923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myrberg AA (2001) The acoustical biology of elasmobranchs. Environ Biol Fishes 60:31–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H (2010) Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 85:935–956

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H (2013) A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol 4:133–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson DR (1967) Hearing thresholds, frequency discrimination, and acoustic orientation in the lemon shark, Negaprion brevirostris (Poey). Bull Mar Sci 17:741–768

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson DR, Johnson RH (1972) Acoustic attraction of Pacific reef sharks: effect of pulse intermittency and variability. Comp Biochem Physiol Part A Physiol 42:85–95

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Porter D, Neuringer A (1984) Music discriminations by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 10:138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powter DM, Gladstone W (2009) Habitat-mediated use of space by juvenile and mating adult Port Jackson sharks, Heterodontus portusjacksoni, in Eastern Australia. Pac Sci 63:1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2016). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford C, Stanley J, Simpson S, Jeffs A (2011) Juvenile coral reef fish use sound to locate habitats. Coral Reefs 30:295–305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remage-Healey L, Nowacek DP, Bass AH (2006) Dolphin foraging sounds suppress calling and elevate stress hormone levels in a prey species, the Gulf toadfish. J Exp Biol 209:4444–4451

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson WJ, Greene CR Jr, Malme CI, Thomson DH (2013) Marine mammals and noise. Academic Press, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan LA, Hart NS, Collin SP, Hemmi JM (2016) Visual resolution and contrast sensitivity in two benthic sharks. J Exp Biol 219:3971–3980

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheipl F, Greven S, Küchenhoff H (2008) Size and power of tests for a zero random effect variance or polynomial regression in additive and linear mixed models. Comput Stat Data Anal 52:3283–3299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schielzeth H, Nakagawa S (2013) rptR: repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data. R package version 0.6. 405/r52

  • Schluessel V (2015) Who would have thought that ‘Jaws’ also has brains? Cognitive functions in elasmobranchs. Anim Cogn 18:19–37

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth SJ (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sih A, Del Giudice M (2012) Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: a behavioural ecology perspective. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 367:2762–2772

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson SD, Meekan M, Montgomery J, McCauley R, Jeffs A (2005) Homeward Sound. Science 308:221–221

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson SD, Meekan MG, Larsen NJ, McCauley RD, Jeffs A (2010) Behavioral plasticity in larval reef fish: orientation is influenced by recent acoustic experiences. Behav Ecol 21:1098–1105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabbekoorn H, Bouton N, van Opzeeland I, Coers A, ten Cate C, Popper AN (2010) A noisy spring: the impact of globally rising underwater sound levels on fish. Trends Ecol Evol 25:419–427

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spedicato GA, Kang TS, Yalamanchi SB, Yadav D (2016) The markovchain package: a package for easily handling Discrete Markov Chains in R. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/markovchain/markovchain.pdf. Accessed 6 Nov 2017

  • Trompf L, Brown C (2014) Personality affects learning and trade-offs between private and social information in guppies, Poecilia reticulata. Anim Behav 88:99–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyack P (1998). Acoustic communication under the sea. Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS (eds) Animal acoustic communication. Springer, New York, pp 163–220

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe S, Sato K (1999) Discriminative stimulus properties of music in Java sparrows. Behav Proc 47:53–57

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • White S, Wagner T, Gowan C, Braithwaite V (2016) Can personality predict individual differences in brook trout spatial learning ability? Behav Process 414:220–228

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson M, Acolas M-L, Bégout M-L, Madsen PT, Wahlberg M (2008) Allis shad (Alosa alosa) exhibit an intensity-graded behavioral response when exposed to ultrasound. J Acoust Soc Am 124:EL243-EL247

    Google Scholar 

  • Zion B, Barki A (2012) Ranching fish using acoustic conditioning: has it reached a dead end? Aquaculture 344–349:3–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zion B, Karplus I, Barki A (2010) Generalization and discrimination of positive and negative acoustic stimuli in the common carp (Cyprinus carpio). Behav Process 83:306–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuur A, Ieno E, Walker N, Saveliev A, Smith G (2009) Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Department of Biological Sciences at Macquarie University, and CVP was supported by an Endeavour Postgraduate (PhD) Scholarship. We thank the members and interns of the Fish Lab and SIMS for husbandry assistance, Jack Gruber for help running the experiment, Drew Allen for advice on statistical analysis, and Vera Schlüessel and two anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Catarina Vila Pouca.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. This work was approved by the Macquarie University Animal Ethics Committee under ARA 2014-003.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vila Pouca, C., Brown, C. Food approach conditioning and discrimination learning using sound cues in benthic sharks. Anim Cogn 21, 481–492 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1183-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1183-1

Keywords

Navigation