Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 20, Issue 5, pp 855–866 | Cite as

The effect of social learning on avoidance of aposematic prey in juvenile great tits (Parus major)

  • Eva Landová
  • Kateřina Hotová Svádová
  • Roman Fuchs
  • Pavel Štys
  • Alice Exnerová
Original Paper

Abstract

Social learning plays an important role in acquiring new foraging skills and food preferences in many bird species but its potential role in learning to avoid aposematic prey has never been studied. We tested the effect of social learning on the acquisition of avoidance of aposematic insect prey (firebug Pyrrhocoris apterus; Heteroptera) in juvenile, hand-reared great tits (Parus major). Behaviour towards aposematic prey was compared between two groups of birds: (1) the observers that were, prior to encounter with firebugs, allowed to watch the experienced conspecific demonstrator repeatedly refuse to attack the prey, and (2) the control birds that lacked this opportunity. Observing an experienced demonstrator was not sufficient for learning complete avoidance, because birds from both groups attacked at least the first firebug they had encountered in avoidance training. However, the opportunity to observe the avoidance behaviour of another bird significantly increased the rate of subsequent individual learning of observers in comparison with control birds. Social learning also decreased mortality of firebugs killed by the birds during the avoidance learning. Socially enhanced learning to avoid aposematic prey might be a mechanism important especially for naive juvenile birds learning from their parents, but it could also enhance learning in adults from their more experienced flock mates. Because social learning of avoidance may also lead to decreased mortality of aposematic prey, its effect should be taken into account in scenarios considering evolution and maintenance of prey warning signals.

Keywords

Parus major Pyrrhocoris apterus Social learning Avoidance learning Aposematic prey Naive predators Warning signalling 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Silvie Rádlová for help with preparation of the figures, Olga Kukal for linguistic advice and corrections, and Daniel Frynta for advice on Bayesian statistics. We are grateful to Ken Cheng, Anders Brodin and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments and suggestions.

Funding

The study was financially supported by Czech Science Foundation (Project P505/11/1459).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of Charles University. We have obtained permission for hand-rearing great tit nestlings (No. 109218/2004/ŽP3/Voj issued by Municipality of Hradec Králové) and for laboratory experiments (Nos. 18847/2003-1020 and 17537/2003-30/300 issued by the Czech Animal Welfare Commission) which cover all methodological details.

References

  1. Adamová-Ježová D, Hospodková E, Fuchsová L, Štys P, Exnerová A (2016) Through experience to boldness? Deactivation of neophobia towards novel and aposematic prey in three European species of tits (Paridae). Behav Process 131:24–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allen T, Clarke JA (2005) Social learning of food preferences by white-tailed ptarmigan chicks. Anim Behav 70:305–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Altshuler DL, Nunn AM (2001) Observational learning in hummingbirds. Auk 118:795–799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Sheldon BC (2012) Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 279:4199–4205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aplin LM, Sheldon BC, Morand-Ferron J (2013) Milk bottles revisited: social learning and individual variation in the blue tit, Cyanistes caeruleus. Anim Behav 85:1225–1232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Cockburn A, Thornton A, Sheldon BC (2015) Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature 518:538–541PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Aronsson M, Gamberale-Stille G (2009) Importance of internal pattern contrast and contrast against the background in aposematic signals. Behav Ecol 20:1356–1362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Aronsson M, Gamberale-Stille G (2012) Colour and pattern similarity in mimicry: evidence for a hierarchical discriminative learning of different components. Anim Behav 84:881–887CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Auersperg AMI, von Bayern AMI, Weber S, Szabadvari A, Bugnyar T, Kacelnik A (2014) Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 281:20140972. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0972 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Barber TA, Klunk AM, Howorth PD, Pearlman MF, Patrick KE (1998) A new look at an old task: advantages and uses of sickness-conditioned learning in day-old chicks. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 60:423–430PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Benskin CWH, Mann NI, Lachlan RF, Slater PJB (2002) Social learning directs feeding preferences in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim Behav 64:823–828CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boogert NJ, Reader SM, Hoppitt W, Laland KN (2008) The origin and spread of innovations in starlings. Anim Behav 75:1509–1518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brodin A, Urhan AU (2014) Interspecific observational memory in non-caching Parus species, the great tit Parus major. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 68:649–656CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brodin A, Urhan AU (2015) Sex differences in learning ability in a common songbird, the great tit-females are better observational learners than males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69:237–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cook M, Mineka S (1990) Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 16:372–389PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cramp S, Perrins CM (1993) Handbook of the birds of Europe, the Middle East and North Africa. The birds of the western Palearctic, vol. VII flycatchers to shrikes. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Curio E, Ernst U, Vieth W (1978) Cultural transmission of enemy recognition: one function of mobbing. Science 202:899–901PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cuthill I, Witter M, Clarke L (1992) The function of bill-wiping. Anim Behav 43:103–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie DA (2007) Handbook of the birds of the world. Vol. 12: 637 Picathartes to tits and chickadees. Lynx Edicions, BarcelonaGoogle Scholar
  20. Ekman J (1989) Ecology of non-breeding social systems of Parus. Wilson Bull 101:263–288Google Scholar
  21. Emery NJ (2006) Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 361:23–43PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Endler JA, Mappes J (2004) Predator mixes and the conspicuousness of the aposematic signal. Am Nat 163:232–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Exnerová A, Landová E, Štys P, Fuchs R, Prokopová M, Cehláriková P (2003) Reactions of passerine birds to aposematic and non-aposematic firebugs (Pyrrhocoris apterus; Heteroptera). Biol J Linn Soc 78:517–525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Exnerová A, Štys P, Fučíková E, Vesela S, Svádová K, Prokopová M, Jarošík V, Fuchs R, Landová E (2007) Avoidance of aposematic prey in european tits (Paridae): learned or innate? Behav Ecol 18:148–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Exnerová A, Svádová KH, Fučíková E, Drent P, Štys P (2010) Personality matters: individual variation in reactions of naïve bird predators to aposematic prey. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 277:723–728CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Exnerová A, Ježová D, Štys P, Doktorovová L, Rojas B, Mappes J (2015) Different reactions to aposematic prey in 2 geographically distant populations of great tits. Behav Ecol 26:1361–1370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Farine JP, Bonnard O, Brossut R, Le Quere JL (1992) Chemistry of defense secretions in nymphs and adults of firebug, Pyrrhocoris apterus L. (Heteroptera, Pyrrhocoridae). J Chem Ecol 18:1673–1682PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Fisher J, Hinde RA (1949) The opening of milk bottles by birds. Br Birds 42:347–357Google Scholar
  29. Forsman A, Merilaita S (1999) Fearful symmetry: pattern size and asymmetry affects aposematic signal efficacy. Evol Ecol 13:131–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Freeberg TM (2004) Social transmission of courtship behaviour and mating preferences in brown-headed cowbirds, Molothrus ater. Learn Behav 32:122–130PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Fryday SL, Greig-Smith PW (1994) The effects of social learning on the food choice of the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Behaviour 128:281–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Galef BG, Laland KN (2005) Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. Bioscience 55:489–499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Galef BG, Wigmore SW (1983) Transfer of information concerning distant foods: a laboratory investigation of the “information-centre” hypothesis. Anim Behav 31:748–758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Galef BG, Wigmore SW, Kennet DJ (1983) A failure to find socially mediated taste aversion learning in Norway rats (R. norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 97:358–363PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Galef BG, McQuoid LM, Whiskin EE (1990) Further evidence that Norway rats do not socially transmit learned aversions to toxic baits. Anim Learn Behav 18:199–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Gamberale G, Tullberg BS (1998) Aposematism and gregariousness: the combined effect of group size and coloration on signal repellence. Proc R Soc Lond B 265:889–894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gamberale-Stille G (2000) Decision time and prey gregariousness influence attack probability in naive and experienced predators. Anim Behav 60:95–99PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gamberale-Stille G, Balogh ACV, Tullberg BS, Leimar O (2011) Feature saltation and the evolution of mimicry. Evolution 66(3):807–817PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Garcia J, Koelling RA (1966) Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychon Sci 4:123–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Learn Behav 32:131–140PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Guilford T (1988) The evolution of conspicuous coloration. Am Nat (Suppl) 131:S7–S21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Guilford T (1994) “Go-slow” signalling and the problem of automimicry. J Theor Biol 170:311–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Guillette LM, Morgan KV, Hall ZJ, Bailey IE, Healy SD (2014) Food preference and copying behaviour in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Behav Process 109:145–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ham AD, Ihalainen E, Lindström L, Mappes J (2006) Does colour matter? The importance of colour in avoidance learning, memorability and generalisation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:482–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Heyes CM (1994) Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev 69:207–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Heyes CM, Galef BG (1996) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  47. Hinde RA, Fisher J (1951) Further observations on the opening of milk bottles by birds. Br Birds 44:392–396Google Scholar
  48. Holzhaider JC, Hunt GR, Gray RD (2010) Social learning in New Caledonian crows. Learn Behav 38:206–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Hotová Svádová K, Exnerová A, Kopečková M, Štys P (2010) Predator dependent mimetic complexes: do passerine birds avoid Central European red-and-black Heteroptera? Eur J Entomol 107:349–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Hotová Svádová K, Exnerová A, Kopečková M, Štys P (2013) How do predators learn to recognize a mimetic complex: experiments with naïve great tits and aposematic Heteroptera. Ethology 119:814–830CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hotová Svádová K, Exnerová A, Štys P (2014) Gregariousness as a defence strategy of moderately defended prey: experiments with Pyrrhocoris apterus and avian predators. Behaviour 151:1617–1640CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. JASP Team (2017) JASP (Version 0.8.1.1); computer softwareGoogle Scholar
  53. Johnston ANB, Burne THJ, Rose SPR (1998) Observational learning in day-old chicks using one-trial passive avoidance learning paradigm. Anim Behav 56:1347–1353PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kitowski I (2009) Social learning of hunting skills in juvenile marsh harriers Circus aeruginosus. J Ethol 27:327–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Lee TJ, Marples NM, Speed MP (2010) Can dietary conservatism explain the primary evolution of aposematism? Anim Behav 79:63–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Lefebvre L (1995) The opening of milk bottles by birds: evidence for accelerating learning rates, but against the wave-of-advance model of cultural transmission. Behav Process 34:43–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Lefebvre L, Bouchard J (2003) Social learning about food in birds. In: Fragaszy DM, Perry S (eds) The biology of traditions: models and evidence. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  58. Lefebvre L, Whittle P, Lascaris E, Finkelstein A (1997) Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Anim Behav 53:549–560CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Lieberman DA (2000) Learning: behavior and cognition, 3rd edn. Belmont, Wadsworth, p 595Google Scholar
  60. Lindstedt C, Lindström L, Mappes J (2008) Hairiness and warning colours as components of antipredator defence: additive or interactive benefits? Anim Behav 75:1703–1713CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Mappes J (1999a) Reactions of hand-reared and wild-caught predators toward warningly colored prey. Behav Ecol 10:317–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Mappes J, Riipi M, Vertainen L (1999b) Can aposematic signals evolve by gradual change? Nature 397:249–251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Lynn SK (2005) Learning to avoid aposematic prey. Anim Behav 70:1221–1226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Mappes J, Marples N, Endler JA (2005) The complex business of survival by aposematism. Trends Ecol Evol 20:598–603PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Marchetti C, Drent PJ (2000) Individual differences in the use of social information in foraging by captive great tits. Anim Behav 60:131–140. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1443 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Marples NM, Kelly DJ (1999) Neophobia and dietary conservatism: two distinct processes? Evol Ecol 13:641–653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Marples NM, Mappes J (2011) Can the dietary conservatism of predators compensate or positive frequency dependent selection against rare, conspicuous prey? Evol Ecol 25:737–749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Marples NM, Roper TJ, Harper DGC (1998) Responses of wild birds to novel prey: evidence of dietary conservatism. Oikos 83:161–165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Marples NM, Quinlan M, Thomas RJ, Kelly DJ (2007) Deactivation of dietary wariness through experience of novel food. Behav Ecol 18:803–810. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm053 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Mason JR, Reidinger RF (1982) Observational learning of food aversions in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). Auk 99:548–554Google Scholar
  71. Mason JR, Arzt AH, Reidinger RF (1984) Comparative assessment of food preferences and aversion acquired by observational learning. Auk 101:796–803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Milgram NW, Krames L, Alloway T (eds) (1977) Food aversion learning. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  73. Pearce JM (2008) Animal learning and cognition. An introduction, 3rd edn. Psychology Press, HoveGoogle Scholar
  74. Riipi M, Alatalo RV, Lindström L, Mappes J (2001) Multiple benefits of gregariousness cover detectability costs in aposematic aggregations. Nature 413:512–514PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Roper TJ, Redston S (1987) Conspicuousness of distasteful prey affects the strength and durability of one-trial avoidance learning. Anim Behav 36:739–747CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Rowe C (2002) Sound improves visual discrimination learning in avian predators. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 269:1353–1357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Rowe C, Guilford T (1996) Hidden colour aversions of domestic chicks triggered by pyrazine odours of insect warning displays. Nature 383:520–522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP (2004) Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford University press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Ruxton GD, Speed MP, Broom M (2007) The importance of initial protection of conspicuous mutants for the coevolution of defense and aposematic signalling of the defense: a modeling study. Evolution 61:2165–2174PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Ruxton GD, Franks DW, Balogh ACV, Leimar O (2008) Evolutionary implications of the form of predator generalization for aposematic signals and mimicry in prey. Evolution 62:2913–2921PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Sasvári L (1979) Observational learning in great, blue and marsh tits. Anim Behav 27:767–771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Sasvári L (1985) Different observational-learning capacity in juvenile and adult individuals of congeneric bird species. Z Tierpsychol 69:293–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Sasvári L (1992) Great tits benefit from feeding in mixed-species flocks: a field experiment. Anim Behav 43:289–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Servedio MR (2000) The effects of predator learning, forgetting and recognition errors on the evolution of warning coloration. Evolution 54:751–763PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Sherry DF, Galef BG Jr (1984) Cultural transmission without imitiation: milk bottle opening by birds. Anim Behav 32:937–938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Sherry DF, Galef BG (1990) Social learning without imitation: more about milk bottle opening by birds. Anim Behav 40:987–989CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Sherwin CM, Heyes CM, Nicol CJ (2002) Social learning influences the preferences of domestic hens for novel food. Anim Behav 63:933–942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Shettleworth SJ (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behaviour, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  89. Siddall EC, Marples NM (2011) The effect of pyrazine odor on avoidance learning and memory in wild robins Erithacus rubecula. Curr Zool 57:208–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Sillén-Tullberg B (1985) Higher survival of an aposematic than of a cryptic form of a distasteful bug. Oecologia 67:411–415PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Skelhorn J (2011) Colour biases are a question of conspecifics’ taste. Anim Behav 81:825–829CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Skelhorn J, Rowe C (2006) Prey palatability influences predator learning and memory. Anim Behav 71:1111–1118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Skelhorn J, Halpin CG, Rowe C (2016) Learning about aposematic prey. Behav Ecol 27(4):955–964CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Smith KE, Halpin CG, Rowe C (2014) Body size matters for aposematic prey during predator aversion learning. Behav Process 109:173–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L (2005) Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. PNAS 102:5460–5465PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Speed MP (2000) Warning signals, receiver psychology and predator memory. Anim Behav 60:269–278PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Speed MP (2001) Can receiver psychology explain the evolution of aposematism? Anim Behav 61:205–216PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Speed M, Ruxton GD, Broom M (2006) Automimicry and the evolution of discrete prey defences. Biol J Linn Soc 87:393–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. StatSoft Inc. (2003) STATISTICA (data analysis software system), version 6Google Scholar
  100. Stevens M, Ruxton GD (2012) Linking the evolution and form of warning coloration in nature. Proc R Soc B 279:419–426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Suboski MD, Bain S, Carty AE, McQuoid LM, Seelen MI, Seifert H (1990) Alarm reaction in acquisition and social transmission of simulated-predator recognition by zebra danio fish (Brachydanio rerio). J Compart Psychol 104:101–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Svádová K, Exnerová A, Štys P, Landová E, Valenta J, Fučíková A, Socha R (2009) Role of different colours of aposematic insects in learning, memory and generalization of naïve bird predators. Anim Behav 77:327–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Templeton JJ, Kamil AC, Balda RP (1999) Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the Pinyon Jay (Gymnorhinus cynocephalus) and the Clark’s Nuncracker (Nucifraga columbiana). J Compart Psychol 113:450–455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Thomas RJ, Marples NM, Cuthill IC, Takahashi M, Gibson EA (2003) Dietary conservatism may facilitate the initial evolution of aposematism. Oikos 101:458–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Verhulst S, Hut RA (1996) Post-fledging care, multiple breeding and the cost of reproduction in the great tit. Anim Behav 51:957–966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Visalberghi E, Addessi E (2000) Response to changes in food palatability in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 59:231–238PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Visalberghi E, Fragaszy DM (1996) Pedagogy and imitation in monkeys: yes, no, or maybe? In: Olson DR, Torrance N (eds) The handbook of education and human development. Blackwell, Cambridge, pp 277–301Google Scholar
  108. Zohar O, Terkel (1996) Social and environmental factors modulate the learning of pine-cone stripping techniques by black rats, Rattus rattus. Anim Behav 51:611–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Zoology, Faculty of ScienceCharles UniversityPrague 2Czech Republic

Personalised recommendations