Animal Cognition

, Volume 20, Issue 2, pp 309–320 | Cite as

Song learning and cognitive ability are not consistently related in a songbird

  • Rindy C. Anderson
  • William A. Searcy
  • Susan Peters
  • Melissa Hughes
  • Adrienne L. DuBois
  • Stephen Nowicki
Original Paper

Abstract

Learned aspects of song have been hypothesized to signal cognitive ability in songbirds. We tested this hypothesis in hand-reared song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) that were tutored with playback of adult songs during the critical period for song learning. The songs developed by the 19 male subjects were compared to the model songs to produce two measures of song learning: the proportion of notes copied from models and the average spectrogram cross-correlation between copied notes and model notes. Song repertoire size, which reflects song complexity, was also measured. At 1 year of age, subjects were given a battery of five cognitive tests that measured speed of learning in the context of a novel foraging task, color association, color reversal, detour-reaching, and spatial learning. Bivariate correlations between the three song measures and the five cognitive measures revealed no significant associations. As in other studies of avian cognition, different cognitive measures were for the most part not correlated with each other, and this result remained true when 22 hand-reared female song sparrows were added to the analysis. General linear mixed models controlling for effects of neophobia and nest of origin indicated that all three song measures were associated with better performance on color reversal and spatial learning but were associated with worse performance on novel foraging and detour-reaching. Overall, the results do not support the hypothesis that learned aspects of song signal cognitive ability.

Keywords

Song learning Cognition Song complexity Songbird Song sparrow Melospiza melodia 

References

  1. Anderson B (1993) Evidence from the rat for a general factor that underlies cognitive performance and that relates to brain size: intelligence? Neurosci Lett 153:98–102CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arcese P, Stoddard PK, Hiebert SM (1988) The form and function of song in female song sparrows. Condor 90:44–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bashi J (1977) Effects of inbreeding on cognitive performance. Nature 266:440–442CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beecher MD, Brenowitz EA (2005) Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends Ecol Evol 20:143–149CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Beecher MD, Burt JM (2004) The role of social interaction in bird song learning. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13:224–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beecher MD, Campbell SE, Stoddard PK (1994) Correlation of song learning and territory establishment strategies in the song sparrow. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 91:1450–1454CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Beecher MD, Burt JM, O’Loghlen AL, Templeton CN, Campbell SE (2007) Bird song learning in an eavesdropping context. Anim Behav 73:929–935CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP (2007) Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica). J Comp Psychol 121:372–379CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Boogert NJ, Giraldeau LA, Lefebvre L (2008) Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Anim Behav 76:1735–1741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boogert NJ, Anderson RC, Peters S, Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2011a) Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Anim Behav 81:1209–1216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boogert NJ, Fawcett TW, Lefebvre L (2011b) Mate choice for cognitive traits: a review of the evidence in nonhuman vertebrates. Behav Ecol 22:447–459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carlson SM, Moses LJ (2001) Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev 72:1032–1053CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Carroll JB (1993) Human cognitive abilities. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dempster FN (1992) The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Dev Rev 12:45–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Diamond A (1990) Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of, inhibitory control in reaching. Ann NY Acad Sci 608:637–669CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF (1991) Male song as a cue for mate choice in the European starling. Behaviour 116:210–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Farrell TM, Weaver K, An YS, MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2012) Song bout length is indicative of spatial learning in European starlings. Behav Ecol 23:101–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Farrell TM, Morgan A, MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2016) Developmental stress impairs performance on an association task in male and female songbirds, but impairs auditory learning in females only. Anim Cogn 19:1–14CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Gallistel CR (2000) The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1179–1191Google Scholar
  21. Galsworthy MJ, Paya-Cano JL, Liu L, Monleón S, Gregoryan G, Fernandes C, Schalkwyk LC et al (2005) Assessing reliability, heritability and general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice. Behav Gen 35:675–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hughes M, Nowicki S, Searcy WA, Peters S (1998) Song-type sharing in song sparrows: implications for repertoire function and song learning. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:437–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Isden J, Panayi C, Dingle C, Madden J (2013) Performance in cognitive and problem-solving tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success. Anim Behav 86:829–838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jacobs LF (1996) Sexual selection and the brain. Trends Ecol Evol 11:82–86CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Keagy J, Savard JF, Borgia G (2009) Male satin bowerbird problem-solving ability predicts mating success. Anim Behav 78:809–817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Keagy J, Savard JF, Borgia G (2011) Complex relationship between multiple measures of cognitive ability and male mating success in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus. Anim Behav 81:1063–1070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kolata S, Light K, Matzel LD (2008) Domain-specific and domain-general learning factors are expressed in genetically heterogeneous CD-1 mice. Intelligence 36:619–629CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Li NP, Bailey JM, Kenrick DT, Linsenmeier JAW (2002) The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: testing the tradeoffs. J Pers Soc Psychol 82:947–955CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Locurto C, Fortin E, Sullivan R (2003) The structure of individual differences in heterogeneous stock mice across problem types and motivational systems. Genes Brain Behav 2:40–55CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn CL, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson RC, Aureli F et al (2014) The evolution of self-control. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 111:E2140–E2148CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Marler P, Mundinger P (1971) Vocal learning in birds. In: Moltz H (ed) Ontogeny of vertebrate behavior. Academic Press, New York, pp 389–450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Marler P, Peters S (1987) A sensitive period for song acquisition in the song sparrow, Melsopiza melodia: a case of age-limited learning. Ethology 76:89–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Marler P, Peters S (1988) The role of song phonology and syntax in vocal learning preferences in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia. Ethology 77:125–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Matzel LD, Han YR, Grossman H, Karnik MS, Patel DN, Scott N, Specht SM et al (2003) Individual differences in the expression of a “general” learning ability in mice. J Neurosci 23:6423–6433PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Miller G (2000) The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. Doubleday, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  36. Nettle D, Andrews CP, Monaghan P, Brilot BO, Bedford T, Gillespie R, Bateson M (2015) Developmental and familial predictors of adult cognitive traits in the European starling. Anim Behav 107:239–248CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Nordby JC, Campbell SE, Beecher MD (1999) Ecological correlates of song learning in song sparrows. Behav Ecol 10:287–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Nordby JC, Campbell SE, Beecher MD (2001) Late song learning in song sparrows. Anim Behav 61:835–846CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Nowicki S, Hasselquist D, Bensch S, Peters S (2000) Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proc Biol Sci 267:2419–2424CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Nowicki S, Searcy WA, Peters S (2002a) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. J Comp Phys A 188:1003–1014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nowicki S, Searcy WA, Peters S (2002b) Quality of song learning affects female response to male bird song. Proc Biol Sci 269:1949–1954CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Nulty B, Burt JM, Akçay Ç, Templeton CN, Campbell SE, Beecher MD (2010) Song learning in song sparrows: relative importance of autumn vs. spring tutoring. Ethology 116:653–661Google Scholar
  43. O’Rourke N, Hatcher L (2013) Factor analysis and structural equation modeling. SAS Institute, CaryGoogle Scholar
  44. Peters S, Searcy WA, Beecher MD, Nowicki S (2000) Geographic variation in the organization of song sparrow repertoires. Auk 117:936–942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Peters S, Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2014) Developmental stress, song learning, and cognition. Integr Comp Biol 54:555–567CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Plomin R (2001) The genetics of G in humans and mouse. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:136–141CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Pravosudov VV, Lavenex P, Omanska A (2005) Nutritional deficits during early development affect hippocampal structure and spatial memory later in life. Behav Neurosci 119:1368–1374CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Prokosch MD, Coss RG, Scheib JE, Blozis SA (2009) Intelligence and mate choice: intelligent men are always appealing. Evol Hum Behav 30:11–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. R Core Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, AustriaGoogle Scholar
  50. Reid JM, Arcese P, Cassidy ALEV, Hiebert SM, Smith JNM, Stoddard PK, Marr AB et al (2004) Song repertoire size predicts initial mating success in male song sparrows, Melospiza melodia. Anim Behav 68:1055–1063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Reid JM, Arcese P, Cassidy ALEV, Marr AB, Smith JNM, Keller LF (2005) Hamilton and Zuk meet heterozygosity? Song repertoire size indicates inbreeding and immunity in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia). Proc Biol Sci 272:481–487CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Riebel K (2009) Song and female mate choice in zebra finches: a review. Adv Study Behav 40:197–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Rowe C, Healy SD (2014) Measuring variation in cognition. Behav Ecol 25:1287–1292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schrijver NCA, Pallier PN, Brown VJ, Würbel H (2004) Double dissociation of social and environmental stimulation on spatial learning and reversal learning in rats. Behav Brain Res 152:307–314CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Searcy WA (1984) Song repertoire size and female preferences in song sparrows. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 14:281–286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Searcy WA, McArthur PD, Yasukawa K (1985) Song repertoire size and male quality in song sparrows. Condor 87:222–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sewall KB, Soha JA, Peters S, Nowicki S (2013) Potential trade-off between vocal ornamentation and spatial ability in a songbird. Biol Lett 9:20130344CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Shaw RC, Boogert NJ, Clayton NS, Burns KC (2015) Wild psychometrics: evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Anim Behav 109:101–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  60. Shettleworth SJ (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behavior, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  61. Shohet AJ, Watt PJ (2009) Female guppies Poecilia reticulata prefer males that can learn fast. J Fish Biol 75:1323–1330CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Spearman C (1904) “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured. Am J Psychol 15:201–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Spencer KA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2011) Indicators of development as sexually selected traits: the developmental stress hypothesis in context. Behav Ecol 22:1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Spritzer MD, Meikle DB, Solomon NG (2005) Female choice based on male spatial ability and aggressiveness among meadow voles. Anim Behav 69:1121–1130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Templeton CN, Laland KN, Boogert NJ (2014) Does song complexity correlate with problem-solving performance in flocks of zebra finches? Anim Behav 92:63–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Timmermans S, Lefebvre L, Boire D, Basu P (2000) Relative size of the hyperstriatum ventrale is the best predictor of feeding innovation rate in birds. Brain Behav Evol 56:196–203CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Vyas A, Harding C, Borg L, Bogdan D (2009) Acoustic characteristics, early experience, and endocrine status interact to modulate female zebra finches’ behavioral responses to songs. Horm Behav 55:50–59CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Biological SciencesFlorida Atlantic UniversityBoca RatonUSA
  2. 2.Department of BiologyUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA
  3. 3.Department of BiologyDuke UniversityDurhamUSA
  4. 4.Department of BiologyCollege of CharlestonCharlestonUSA

Personalised recommendations