Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 19, Issue 5, pp 921–937 | Cite as

Are apes essentialists? Scope and limits of psychological essentialism in great apes

  • Trix Cacchione
  • Christine Hrubesch
  • Josep Call
  • Hannes Rakoczy
Original Paper

Abstract

Human reasoning is characterized by psychological essentialism (Gelman in The essential child: origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford University Press, New York, 2003): when reasoning about objects, we distinguish between deep essential properties defining the object’s kind and identity, and merely superficial features that can be changed without altering the object’s identity. To date, it is unclear whether psychological essentialism is based on the acquisition of linguistic means (such as kind terms) and therefore uniquely human, or whether it is a more fundamental cognitive capacity which might be present also in the absence of language. In the present study, we addressed this question by testing whether, and if so, under which circumstances non-human apes also rely on psychological essentialism to identify objects. For this purpose, we adapted classical verbal transformation scenarios used in research on psychological essentialism (Keil in Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. MIT Press, Cambridge, 1989) and implemented them in two nonverbal tasks: first, a box task, typically used to test object individuation (Experiment 1), and second, an object choice task, typically used to test object discrimination, object preferences and logical inferences (Experiments 2–4). Taken together, the results of the four experiments suggest that under suitable circumstances (when memory and other task demands are minimized), great apes engage in basic forms of essentialist reasoning. Psychological essentialism is thus possible also in the absence of language.

Keywords

Object individuation Essentialism Comparative cognition Categorization Conceptual development 

Supplementary material

Supplementary material 1 (MP4 14317 kb)

Supplementary material 2 (MP4 15057 kb)

References

  1. Anderson JR, Sallaberry P, Barbier H (1995) Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 49:201–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bräuer J, Call J (2011) The magic cup: great apes and domestic dogs individuate objects according to their properties. J Comp Psychol 125:353–361CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Cacchione T, Schaub S, Rakoczy H (2013) Fourteen-month-old infants infer the continuous identity of objects on the basis of non-visible causal properties. Dev Psychol 49:1325–1329CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Cohen J, Cohen P (1983) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  5. Deak GO (2006) Do children really confuse appearance and reality? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:546–550CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. DeLoache J (2000) Dual representation and young children’s use of scale models. Child Dev 71:329–338CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Fontanari L, Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2011) Object individuation in 3-day-old chicks: use of property and spatiotemporal information. Dev Sci 14:1235–1244CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Fontanari L, Rugani R, Regolin L, Vallortigara G (2014) Use of kind information for object individuation in young domestic chicks. Anim Cogn 17:925–935CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gelman SA (2003) The essential child: origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gelman SA, Markman EM (1986) Categories and induction in young children. Cognition 23:183–209CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gelman SA, Markman EM (1987) Young children’s inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. Child Dev 58:1532–1541CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Gelman SA, Wellman HM (1991) Insides and essences: early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition 38:213–244CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gottfried GM, Gelman SA (2005) Developing domain-specific causal-explanatory frameworks: the role of insides and immanence. Cognitive Development 20:137–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Herrnstein RJ, Loveland DH (1964) Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science 146:549–551CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Herrnstein RJ, Loveland DH, Cable C (1976) Natural concepts in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2:285–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Keil FC (1982) Intelligence and the rest of cognition. Intelligence 6:1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Keil FC (1989) Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  18. Krachun C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Can chimpanzees distinguish appearance from reality? Cognition 112:435–450CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Kripke S (1972) Naming and necessity. In: Davidson D, Harman G (eds) Semantic of natural language. Reidel, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  20. Krøjgaard P (2004) A review of object individuation in infancy. Br J Dev Psychol 22:159–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Medin D (1989) Concepts and conceptual structure. Am Psychol 44:1469–1481CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Medin D, Ortony A (1989) Psychological essentialism. In: Vosniadou S, Ortony A (eds) Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 179–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mendes N, Rakoczy H, Call J (2008) Ape metaphysics: object individuation without language. Cognition 106:730–749CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Mendes N, Rakoczy H, Call J (2011) Primates do not spontaneously use shape properties for object individuation: a competence or a performance problem? Anim Cogn 14:407–414CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Needham A, Baillargeon R (2000) Infants’ use of featural and experiential information in segregating and individuating objects: a reply to Xu, Carey, and Welch (1999). Cognition 74:255–284CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Newman GE, Herrmann P, Wynn K, Keil FC (2008) Biases towards internal features in infants’ reasoning about objects. Cognition 107:420–432CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Pepperberg IM (2013) Abstract concepts: data from a Grey parrot. Behav Process 93:82–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Phillips W, Santos LR (2007) Evidence for kind representations in the absence of language: experiments with rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Cognition 102:455–463CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Phillips W, Shankar M, Santos LR (2010) Essentialism in the absence of language? Evidence from rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Dev Sci 13:F1–F7CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Putnam H (1975) The meaning of meaning. In: Putnam H (ed) Mind, language and reality. Cambridge University Press, LondonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Quine WVO (1960) Word and object. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  32. Rakoczy H, Cacchione T (2014) The developmental and evolutionary origins of psychological essentialism lie in sortal object individuation. Behav Brain Sci 37:500–501PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Sanchez A, Pereto M, Call J (2016) Differences in between-reinforcer value modulate the selective-value effect in great apes (Pan troglodyes, P. paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii). J Comp Psychol 130(1):1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Santos LR, Sulkowski GM, Spaepen GM, Hauser MD (2002) Object individuation using property/kind information in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Cognition 83:241–264CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Seed A, Seddon E, Greene B, Call J (2012) Chimpanzees ‘folk physics’: bringing failures into focus. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:2743–2752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Shutts K, Condry KF, Santos LR, Spelke ES (2009) Core knowledge and its limits: the domain of food. Cognition 112:120–140CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Uller C, Carey S, Hauser M, Xu F (1997) Is language needed for constructing sortal concepts? A study with nonhuman primates. In: Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development. Cascadilla Press, Somerville, pp 665–677Google Scholar
  38. van de Walle GA, Carey S, Prevor M (2000) Bases for object individuation in infancy: evidence from manual search. J Cogn Dev 1:249–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Xu F (2002) The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition 85:223–250CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Xu F (2005) Categories, kinds, and object individuation in infancy. In: Gershkoff-Stowe G, Rakison D (eds) Building object categories in developmental time. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 63–89Google Scholar
  41. Xu F (2007) Language acquisition and concept formation: count nouns and object kinds. In: Gaskell G (ed) Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 627–634Google Scholar
  42. Xu F, Baker A (2005) Object individuation in 10-month-old infants using a simplified manual search method. J Cogn Dev 6:307–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Xu F, Carey S, Quint N (2004) The emergence of kind-based object individuation in infancy. Cogn Psychol 49:155–190CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Yerkes RM, Dodson JD (1908) The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 18:459–482CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Trix Cacchione
    • 1
    • 6
  • Christine Hrubesch
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Josep Call
    • 2
    • 4
  • Hannes Rakoczy
    • 5
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of BerneBernSwitzerland
  2. 2.Department of Developmental and Comparative PsychologyMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
  3. 3.Anthropological Institute and MuseumUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  4. 4.School of Psychology and NeuroscienceUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
  5. 5.Department of Developmental PsychologyUniversity of GoettingenGoettingenGermany
  6. 6.University of Applied Sciences and Arts Northwestern SwitzerlandBruggSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations